2011
DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0025151
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Context Modulates the ERP Signature of Contour Integration

Abstract: We investigated how the electrophysiological signature of contour integration is changed by the context in which a contour is embedded. Specifically, we manipulated the orientations of Gabor elements surrounding an embedded shape outline. The amplitudes of early visual components over posterior scalp regions were changed by the presence of a contour, and by the orientation of elements surrounding the contour. Differences in context type had an effect on the early P1 and N1 components, but not on the later P2 c… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

4
17
0

Year Published

2013
2013
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 20 publications
(21 citation statements)
references
References 36 publications
(47 reference statements)
4
17
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Executing of this task suggests activation of mechanisms of visual integration by grouping spatially integrated elements into a global structure rather than discrimination of the local features as in the Collinearity task. A similar increase in the negative wave of ERP in this time window was observed when a global Gabor contour was embedded in an array of randomly oriented Gabor patches (Butler et al, 2013;Machilsen, Novitskiy, Vancleef, & Wagemans, 2011;Shpaner, Molholm, Forde, & Foxe, 2013). An increase in the N1-amplitude peaking $100-170 ms after stimulus onset is also often found when participants observe an illusory contour such as a Kanizsa square which is associated with the emergence of a global shape (Herrmann & Bosch, 2001;Murray et al, 2002;Proverbio & Zani, 2002).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 67%
“…Executing of this task suggests activation of mechanisms of visual integration by grouping spatially integrated elements into a global structure rather than discrimination of the local features as in the Collinearity task. A similar increase in the negative wave of ERP in this time window was observed when a global Gabor contour was embedded in an array of randomly oriented Gabor patches (Butler et al, 2013;Machilsen, Novitskiy, Vancleef, & Wagemans, 2011;Shpaner, Molholm, Forde, & Foxe, 2013). An increase in the N1-amplitude peaking $100-170 ms after stimulus onset is also often found when participants observe an illusory contour such as a Kanizsa square which is associated with the emergence of a global shape (Herrmann & Bosch, 2001;Murray et al, 2002;Proverbio & Zani, 2002).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 67%
“…Additionally, these components are sensitive to grouping and closure phenomena [7], [8], [28], [34][36], and the P2 has been observed to be modulated by perceptual implicit memory [37]. Taking these findings together, the P2 appears to index an intermediate processing stage that is closely bound to perceptual processes such as segmentation or grouping and higher-level processes such as categorization.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 91%
“…Grouping by proximity was associated with a positive component over occipital electrodes that peaked between 100 and 120 ms after sensory stimulation. Similarly, contour integration by collinearity emerges from 130 ms after stimulus onset, as evidenced by the results from Machilsen et al (2011). In contrast, grouping by similarity (in shape or colour) was linked to a negativity over occipito-temporal scalp regions occurring much later, around 300 ms (Han, 2004;Han and Humphreys, 2007;Han et al, 2001Han et al, , 2002Han et al, , 2005aMao et al, 2004).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 80%