2017
DOI: 10.1037/lhb0000216
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Context influences interpretation of eyewitness confidence statements.

Abstract: When an eyewitness makes an identification from a lineup, he or she is asked to provide a confidence statement to help jurors assess credibility. However, these are verbal statements and people must rely on metacognitive processes to correctly interpret them. Recently, Dodson and Dobolyi (2015) argued that a person's interpretation of a witness's verbal confidence is influenced by the diagnosticity of the features used to justify his or her identification. We tested this hypothesis in 2 experiments. Experiment… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1

Citation Types

4
47
2

Year Published

2019
2019
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 23 publications
(53 citation statements)
references
References 26 publications
4
47
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Finally, it is worth explaining why the effect-size for the featural justification effect in the single eyewitness conditions (e.g., Conf-only vs. Feat-only) in Experiment 1 is smaller than what has been observed in previous studies (Cash & Lane, 2017;Dobolyi & Dodson, 2018;Dodson & Dobolyi, 2015. In previous studies, participants saw eight lineups, whereas in Experiment 1 they saw a single lineup.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 72%
“…Finally, it is worth explaining why the effect-size for the featural justification effect in the single eyewitness conditions (e.g., Conf-only vs. Feat-only) in Experiment 1 is smaller than what has been observed in previous studies (Cash & Lane, 2017;Dobolyi & Dodson, 2018;Dodson & Dobolyi, 2015. In previous studies, participants saw eight lineups, whereas in Experiment 1 they saw a single lineup.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 72%
“…While there is much information about the value that confidence holds when a numeric rating scale is used, less is known about the diagnostic value of verbal confidence statements (Behrman & Richards, 2005;Cash & Lane, 2017;2021;Dobolyi & Dodson, 2018;Dodson & Dobolyi, 2015;Grabman, Dobolyi, Berelovich, & Dodson, 2019;Klobuchar, Steblay, & Caliguiri, 2006;Mansour, 2020). Wixted and Wells (2017) highlighted this as a priority for future research, about which they asked: Should a confidence statement be taken in the witness's own words (as in Klobuchar et al, 2006), or should confidence be recorded using an explicit 3-point rating scale (as in Wixted et al, 2016) -or should a 100-point scale be used?…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although there is much information about the value that confidence holds when a numeric rating scale is used, less is known about the diagnostic value of verbal confidence statements (Behrman & Richards, 2005;Cash & Lane, 2017;Dobolyi & Dodson, 2018;Dodson & Dobolyi, 2015;Klobuchar et al, 2006;Mansour, 2020). Wixted and Wells (2017) highlighted this as a priority for future research, about which they asked:…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…While there is much information about the value that confidence holds when a rating scale is used, less is known about the diagnostic value that verbal confidence statements hold (Behrman & Richards, 2005;Cash & Lane, 2017;Dobolyi & Dodson, 2018;Dodson & Dobolyi, 2015;Grabman, Dobolyi, Berelovich, & Dodson, 2019;Klobuchar, Steblay, & Caliguiri, 2006;Mansour, 2020). This is why Wixted and Wells (2017) highlighted this as a priority for future research, about which they asked: Should a confidence statement be taken in the witness's own words (as in Klobuchar et al, 2006), or should confidence be recorded using an explicit 3-point rating scale (as in Wixted et al, 2016) -or should a 100-point scale be used?…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%