2007
DOI: 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2007.02.004
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Constructions as categories of language

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

5
67
2
3

Year Published

2008
2008
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 63 publications
(83 citation statements)
references
References 54 publications
5
67
2
3
Order By: Relevance
“…This weighting explains the finding that a slot that is highly skewed towards one particular filler largely takes on the properties of that filler (Casenhiser and Goldberg 2005;Goldberg, Casenhiser, and Sethuraman, 2004;Goldberg, Casenhiser, and White, 2007). For example, the properties of the "VERB" slot in the double-object dative construction (not just semantic, but also morphophonological) are largely those of give, which accounts for the lion's share of all occurrences of this construction (Ambridge, Pine, Rowland, Freudenthal, and Chang, 2014).…”
Section: Appendix: the "Weighted Average"mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This weighting explains the finding that a slot that is highly skewed towards one particular filler largely takes on the properties of that filler (Casenhiser and Goldberg 2005;Goldberg, Casenhiser, and Sethuraman, 2004;Goldberg, Casenhiser, and White, 2007). For example, the properties of the "VERB" slot in the double-object dative construction (not just semantic, but also morphophonological) are largely those of give, which accounts for the lion's share of all occurrences of this construction (Ambridge, Pine, Rowland, Freudenthal, and Chang, 2014).…”
Section: Appendix: the "Weighted Average"mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Because the constraints are functional, and functionality is itself context-dependent and graded, they will emerge and be used in context-dependent and graded manners. In consequence, there is no clean cut between syntax and meaning (Goldberg et al 2007). Various other kinds of functional considerations, such as processing demands and statistical properties of exposure, will be functionally relevant for what is classically taken to be in the realm of austere mathematical syntax (Diessel 2007;Hawkins 2007).…”
Section: Properties and Consequencesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Prior construction learning studies (Boyd, Gottschalk, and Goldberg 2009;Casenhiser and Goldberg 2005;Goldberg and Casenhiser 2008;Goldberg, Casenhiser, and Sethuraman 2004;Goldberg, Casenhiser, and White 2007) have shown that English L1 speakers can comprehend and produce the novel construction of appearance (N 1 N 2 V), with the corresponding meaning of N 1 "appears in/on" N 2 (e.g., the spot the king moopoed) after relatively brief exposure. In contrast, construction learning studies with L2 English speakers reported greater difficulty in detecting a variety of constructions, including the appearance construction and the Samoan ergative construction (Nakamura 2012), English dative constructions (McDonough and Nekrasova-Becker 2014;Year and Gordon 2009), and Esperanto transitives (Fulga and McDonough 2014;McDonough and Fulga 2015;McDonough and Trofimovich 2013).…”
Section: Challenges In Detecting Novel Constructionsmentioning
confidence: 99%