The Handbook of Language Emergence 2015
DOI: 10.1002/9781118346136.ch22
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A Constructivist Account of Child Language Acquisition

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
50
0
3

Year Published

2015
2015
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
7
3

Relationship

2
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 49 publications
(54 citation statements)
references
References 161 publications
1
50
0
3
Order By: Relevance
“…The dilemma again is one of lumping versus splitting; and the notion of a single abstract SVO transitive construction is perhaps the most extreme and inaccurate example of lumping that we have encountered so far. Consider the various different sentence types that must be subsumed by a unitary SVO transitive construction (adapted from Ambridge & Lieven, 2015 What is particularly problematic in this case is that certain concrete instantiations of this would-be unitary construction are not just radically different, but polar opposites (e.g. John feared Bill vs John frightened Bill).…”
Section: Sentence-level Constructionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The dilemma again is one of lumping versus splitting; and the notion of a single abstract SVO transitive construction is perhaps the most extreme and inaccurate example of lumping that we have encountered so far. Consider the various different sentence types that must be subsumed by a unitary SVO transitive construction (adapted from Ambridge & Lieven, 2015 What is particularly problematic in this case is that certain concrete instantiations of this would-be unitary construction are not just radically different, but polar opposites (e.g. John feared Bill vs John frightened Bill).…”
Section: Sentence-level Constructionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Some argue that purely distributional learning mechanisms could discover these high‐order abstractions precluding the need for analogical abstraction mechanisms (e.g., Twomey, Chang, & Ambridge, ; see Ambridge & Lieven, ; Ambridge, Goldwater, & Lieven, unpublished data); however, they need not be mutually exclusive as sequential‐statistical learning mechanisms, and mechanisms of analogical abstraction work together in other domains. For example, one could independently learn via the co‐occurrence statistics of causes and effects that two different natural phenomena are characterized by multiple causes causing a single effect (e.g., Fernbach & Sloman, ; Holyoak & Cheng, ), and then structural alignment can support recognizing this commonality in the causal relations between the two phenomena (Goldwater & Gentner, ).…”
Section: How Relational Learning Fits Into Domain‐general Accounts Ofmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A number of scholars have staked out positions that place a maximum emphasis on the learner's sensitivity to language use by others. Often labeled the usage‐based theory of learning (Ambridge & Lieven, ; MacWhinney, ; Tomasello, ), this approach holds that learning arises through “language use itself, by means of social skills like joint attention, and by means of powerful generalization mechanisms” that are highly sensitive to experience (Behrens, , p. 383). Soderstrom, Conwell, Feldman, and Morgan () go so far as to call this perspective “the new paradigm of language acquisition” (p. 409).…”
Section: Developmentmentioning
confidence: 99%