1953
DOI: 10.1126/science.118.3072.598
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Conformity to Social Norms and Attraction to the Group

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
7
0

Year Published

1958
1958
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 17 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 3 publications
0
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…A second goal was to begin to look at possible positive and negative modeling mechanisms, focusing in particular on how much the participant likes the model. As previously noted, individuals tend to like people who are similar to themselves (e.g., C. D. Johnson, Gormly, & Gormly, 1973;Singh & Ho, 2000), and other work suggests that individuals may conform more with those they like, in part for acceptance (Bovard, 1953;Goethals & Nelson, 1973;Kiesler & Corbin, 1965;Rotter, 1967;Singh & Ho, 2000) and in part perhaps to maintain balance or cognitive consistency among cognitions (e.g., Heider, 1958;Newcomb, 1968;Rosenberg & Abelson, 1960). Heider's (1958) balance theory perspective, for example, would hold that if a participant in our first study liked the model, the participant should have felt pressure to become positive toward the musical selection made by the model, in order to achieve balance or consistency among the cognitive elements, namely, positive relationships among participant, model, and music.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 86%
“…A second goal was to begin to look at possible positive and negative modeling mechanisms, focusing in particular on how much the participant likes the model. As previously noted, individuals tend to like people who are similar to themselves (e.g., C. D. Johnson, Gormly, & Gormly, 1973;Singh & Ho, 2000), and other work suggests that individuals may conform more with those they like, in part for acceptance (Bovard, 1953;Goethals & Nelson, 1973;Kiesler & Corbin, 1965;Rotter, 1967;Singh & Ho, 2000) and in part perhaps to maintain balance or cognitive consistency among cognitions (e.g., Heider, 1958;Newcomb, 1968;Rosenberg & Abelson, 1960). Heider's (1958) balance theory perspective, for example, would hold that if a participant in our first study liked the model, the participant should have felt pressure to become positive toward the musical selection made by the model, in order to achieve balance or consistency among the cognitive elements, namely, positive relationships among participant, model, and music.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 86%
“…Other social scientists (mostly, but not exclusively, social psychologists) have instead been more interested in understanding the cognitive processes that give origin to social norms. They studied social norms as beliefs that originate from observing what people do and like (Bovard 1953a, Griskevicius et al . , Mackie et al .…”
Section: Key Differences Between Social and Gender Normsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…An alternative sequence may be seen to occur as well: motivation having reference to some fulfillment through the group serves to heighten the individual's perception of its expectancies. 4…”
Section: Norms Roles and Group Expectanciesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This view cuts across a number of other motivational schema suggested elsewhere (cf. 4,8,9,12,23), and is intended more as a resolution than a departure. Briefly, these other distinctions appear to involve an "activity focus" and an "other people focus."…”
Section: Individual Variablesmentioning
confidence: 99%