2010
DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.02.043
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Conflict and inhibition differentially affect the N200/P300 complex in a combined go/nogo and stop-signal task

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

27
271
2
1

Year Published

2011
2011
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 319 publications
(301 citation statements)
references
References 49 publications
27
271
2
1
Order By: Relevance
“…N2 amplitude did not differ between go and no-go trials when the target stimuli were presented within congruent (low conflict) arrays (RRR, NNN) but was significantly greater on incongruent than congruent flanker trials across trial types, supporting the assertion that this particular ERP is better conceptualised as a marker of response conflict than response 20 inhibition (Brydges et al, 2012;Donkers & van Boxtel, 2004;Enriquez-Geppert et al, 2010;Randall & Smith, 2011;Smith et al, 2010;van Veen & Carter, 2002). The task employed in the present study equated the frequency of go and no-go trials; the findings therefore provide further support for previous studies in which N2 amplitude is not enhanced on nogo trials when these are equally frequent to go trials (Donkers & van Boxtel, 2004;EnriquezGeppert et al, 2010;Nieuwenhuis et al, 2003).…”
Section: N2 Amplitudementioning
confidence: 69%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…N2 amplitude did not differ between go and no-go trials when the target stimuli were presented within congruent (low conflict) arrays (RRR, NNN) but was significantly greater on incongruent than congruent flanker trials across trial types, supporting the assertion that this particular ERP is better conceptualised as a marker of response conflict than response 20 inhibition (Brydges et al, 2012;Donkers & van Boxtel, 2004;Enriquez-Geppert et al, 2010;Randall & Smith, 2011;Smith et al, 2010;van Veen & Carter, 2002). The task employed in the present study equated the frequency of go and no-go trials; the findings therefore provide further support for previous studies in which N2 amplitude is not enhanced on nogo trials when these are equally frequent to go trials (Donkers & van Boxtel, 2004;EnriquezGeppert et al, 2010;Nieuwenhuis et al, 2003).…”
Section: N2 Amplitudementioning
confidence: 69%
“…According to this model, the N2 is modulated in inhibitory control tasks because the ratio of go to no-go or stop trials creates conflict between the prepotent response tendency and the infrequent requirement to inhibit the response (Braver, Barch, Gray, Molfese, & Snyder, 2001), not because of inhibition per se. In support of this, N2 amplitude is greater on go than no-go trials when the ratio of go: no-go trials is reversed (Donkers & van Boxtel, 2004;Enriquez-Geppert et al, 2010;Nieuwenhuis, Yeung, van den Wildenberg, & Ridderinkhof, 2003) and on incongruent than congruent flanker trials in the visual flanker 6 task (Bartholow et al, 2005;Clayson & Larson, 2011;Purmann, Badde, Luna-Rodriguez, & Wendt, 2011). In the flanker task participants must respond to a central target stimulus while simultaneously suppressing an opposing response associated with the flanking stimuli.…”
Section: The N2 Event-related Potentialmentioning
confidence: 72%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Several authors have assumed that inhibition in this time window is a top-down executive control process (Ridderinkhof et al 1999;Enriquez-Geppert et al 2010). As previously described, the Go/No-go and Stop-signal paradigms are frequently used to study inhibition.…”
Section: Automatic and Controlled Nature Of Inhibition Between 200 Anmentioning
confidence: 99%