2021
DOI: 10.1093/sleep/zsab249
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Concordance of multiple methods to define resiliency and vulnerability to sleep loss depends on Psychomotor Vigilance Test metric

Abstract: Study Objectives Sleep restriction (SR) and total sleep deprivation (TSD) reveal well-established individual differences in Psychomotor Vigilance Test (PVT) performance. While prior studies have used different methods to categorize such resiliency/vulnerability, none have systematically investigated whether these methods categorize individuals similarly. Methods 41 adults participated in a 13-day laboratory study consisting o… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

3
14
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

4
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 8 publications
(17 citation statements)
references
References 61 publications
3
14
0
Order By: Relevance
“…During sleep deprivation and psychological stress, the range of the Spearman relative rank correlations between the two objective performance measures (3-min PVT and DSST) was ρ = −0.579, p = 0.001, and the range between the objective performance measures and the KSS was ρ = −0.142–0.371, p = 0.036–0.439. The profiles of change in NTB performance and self-report scores with sleep loss and psychological stress are comparable to results obtained in laboratory studies ( Dennis et al, 2017 ; Yamazaki and Goel, 2020 ; Brieva et al, 2021 ; Casale et al, 2021b ; Yamazaki et al, 2021a , b ).…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 81%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…During sleep deprivation and psychological stress, the range of the Spearman relative rank correlations between the two objective performance measures (3-min PVT and DSST) was ρ = −0.579, p = 0.001, and the range between the objective performance measures and the KSS was ρ = −0.142–0.371, p = 0.036–0.439. The profiles of change in NTB performance and self-report scores with sleep loss and psychological stress are comparable to results obtained in laboratory studies ( Dennis et al, 2017 ; Yamazaki and Goel, 2020 ; Brieva et al, 2021 ; Casale et al, 2021b ; Yamazaki et al, 2021a , b ).…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 81%
“…As such, resilient and vulnerable individuals were determined by a median split on average values from the three NTB sessions during TSD ( Patanaik et al, 2015 ; Moreno-Villanueva et al, 2018 ; Caldwell et al, 2020 ) for 3-min PVT total lapses and errors, DSST performance, and KSS scores. We dichotomized participants as such given that, for initial examination and categorization of novel biomarkers, it is more suitable and applicable to create resilient and vulnerable groups in healthy adult samples, as per convention in the field ( Chuah et al, 2009 ; Rocklage et al, 2009 ; Chee and Tan, 2010 ; Diekelmann et al, 2010 ; Patanaik et al, 2015 ; Yeo et al, 2015 ; Xu et al, 2016 ; Moreno-Villanueva et al, 2018 ; Caldwell et al, 2020 ; Salfi et al, 2020 ; Brieva et al, 2021 ; Casale et al, 2021b ; Yamazaki et al, 2021b ), especially given our sample size. Importantly, systematic examination of multiple approaches and thresholds for evaluating differential neurobehavioral vulnerability to sleep loss has demonstrated that median splits on averaged performance scores, rather than change from baseline or variance in scores, are consistent indicators of resilience and vulnerability during sleep deprived and well-rested periods ( Brieva et al, 2021 ; Casale et al, 2021b ; Yamazaki et al, 2021b ), thus further justifying our methods.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Chronic sleep deprivation is a significant public health issue and is associated with multiple adverse health risks such as cardiovascular disease, obesity, diabetes, cancer, and overall morbidity and mortality ( Ferrie et al, 2007 ; Gallicchio and Kalesan, 2009 ; Mullington et al, 2009 ). For many individuals, sleep loss increases self-rated sleepiness and deficits in sustained attention ( Banks and Dinges, 2007 ; Goel et al, 2009 ; Brieva et al, 2021 ; Casale et al, 2021 ; Yamazaki et al, 2021a , b ). However, large, highly replicable, phenotypic individual differences occur in response to sleep deprivation, whereby some individuals are vulnerable, and others are resilient to sleep loss ( Van Dongen et al, 2004 ; Goel, 2017 ; Yamazaki and Goel, 2020 ; Brieva et al, 2021 ; Casale et al, 2021 ; Yamazaki et al, 2021a , b ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For many individuals, sleep loss increases self-rated sleepiness and deficits in sustained attention ( Banks and Dinges, 2007 ; Goel et al, 2009 ; Brieva et al, 2021 ; Casale et al, 2021 ; Yamazaki et al, 2021a , b ). However, large, highly replicable, phenotypic individual differences occur in response to sleep deprivation, whereby some individuals are vulnerable, and others are resilient to sleep loss ( Van Dongen et al, 2004 ; Goel, 2017 ; Yamazaki and Goel, 2020 ; Brieva et al, 2021 ; Casale et al, 2021 ; Yamazaki et al, 2021a , b ). These inter-individual differences are robust for common types of sleep loss, such as total sleep deprivation (TSD) and chronic sleep restriction (SR) ( Dennis et al, 2017 ; Yamazaki and Goel, 2020 ), persisting across months and years ( Dennis et al, 2017 ), but do not differ between various subgroups, such as age, sex, race, and body mass index ( Yamazaki and Goel, 2020 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%