2010
DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.02.003
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Competing demands of prosociality and equity in monkeys

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
39
0

Year Published

2011
2011
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
4
4

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 52 publications
(40 citation statements)
references
References 37 publications
1
39
0
Order By: Relevance
“…table 1]. Moreover, in the wild, coalitions, which are a form of cooperative behavior, are not observed, and cooperation seems to be maintained despite unequal outcomes between the participants [Chalmeau et al, 1997], an outcome which is in contrast to chimpanzees [Melis et al, 2006] and capuchins [Brosnan et al, 2006;Brosnan et al, 2010a]. It may be that orangutans are capable of cooperation, but do not normally choose to do so, possibly a secondary feature derived from their far less gregarious social organization.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…table 1]. Moreover, in the wild, coalitions, which are a form of cooperative behavior, are not observed, and cooperation seems to be maintained despite unequal outcomes between the participants [Chalmeau et al, 1997], an outcome which is in contrast to chimpanzees [Melis et al, 2006] and capuchins [Brosnan et al, 2006;Brosnan et al, 2010a]. It may be that orangutans are capable of cooperation, but do not normally choose to do so, possibly a secondary feature derived from their far less gregarious social organization.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Even in studies that found evidence for prosocial tendencies, the effects sizes are often rather small (e.g., Brosnan et al, 2010a;Burkart et al, 2007;Lakshminarayanan and Santos, 2008), which may explain the low replication rate. However, given a presumed publication bias for positive results, these findings should be interpreted with caution.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…Capuchin monkeys barely differentiated in an instrumental helping paradigm between a partner being present or absent both when they were presented with food rewards and when they received tokens that could be exchanged for food (Brosnan et al, 2010a). However, it is possible that in some conditions a ceiling effect masked a difference between the social test condition and the no-partner control condition.…”
Section: Instrumental Helpingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…There is evidence that they notice the discrepancy (Brosnan, Talbot et al 2010), and it may be that our experimental designs are not conducive to eliciting such prosocial responses (Murphy and Miller 1955). In particular, as discussed above, despite chimpanzees' apparent disinterest in their partners' outcomes (Silk, Brosnan et al 2005;Jensen, Hare et al 2006), capuchin monkeys are quite responsive (de Waal, Leimgruber et al 2008), even to the point of bringing their partner more than they will receive (Lakshminarayanan and Santos 2008;Brosnan, Houser et al 2010). Thus, future work is needed to delineate the magnitude of this apparent difference between humans and other primates.…”
Section: Comparing Human and Non-human Primatesmentioning
confidence: 93%
“…However, it soon became clear that there was more to the story. Capuchin monkeys, for instance, routinely provide benefits to partners (de Waal, Leimgruber et al 2008), even when their partner stands to gain more than they do (Lakshminarayanan and Santos 2008;Brosnan, Houser et al 2010). Moreover, cooperatively breeding callithrichids, who share parental duties among the mated pair as well as unweaned offspring, typically behave prosocially (Burkart, Fehr et al 2007;Cronin, Schroeder et al 2010) (although this is not always the casesee Cronin, Schroeder et al 2009;Stevens 2010).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%