2011
DOI: 10.1111/j.2044-8309.2011.02022.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Collective self and individual choice: The effects of inter‐group comparative context on environmental values and behaviour

Abstract: Self-categorization theory suggests that inter-group comparisons inform individual behaviour by affecting perceived in-group stereotypes that are internalized by group members. The present paper provides evidence for this chain of effects in the domain of environmental behaviour. In two studies, inter-group comparative context was manipulated. Study 1 found that the perceived in-group stereotype, self-stereotype (as represented by the reported value centrality), and behavioural intentions shifted away from a c… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
47
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
2
1

Relationship

1
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 70 publications
(50 citation statements)
references
References 55 publications
3
47
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Salience had no effect on liberals who reported strong climate-friendly beliefs and support across conditions. In addition, Rabinovich, Morton, Postmes, and Verplanken ( 2011 ) varied the intergroup context to examine its influence on sustainable behaviour. In their work, British participants were asked to write down the ways that being British differed from being Swedish (an upward climate action comparison) or from being American (a downward climate comparison).…”
Section: Social Identity Perspectivesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Salience had no effect on liberals who reported strong climate-friendly beliefs and support across conditions. In addition, Rabinovich, Morton, Postmes, and Verplanken ( 2011 ) varied the intergroup context to examine its influence on sustainable behaviour. In their work, British participants were asked to write down the ways that being British differed from being Swedish (an upward climate action comparison) or from being American (a downward climate comparison).…”
Section: Social Identity Perspectivesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, research from this perspective has shown that (a) identity is fluid and context-dependent (e.g., Onorato & Turner, 2002); (b) social forces (e.g., comparative contexts, intergroup relations) shape how individuals understand the defining features of groups (e.g., Haslam, Turner, Oakes, McGarty, & Hayes, 1992), including those to which they belong (e.g., Simon, 1992), and this has consequences for how they understand their self and act on that basis (e.g., Rabinovich, Morton, Postmes, & Verplanken, 2012), and; (c) people's evaluations of, and emotional reactions to, other people, information, ideas, and events varies as a function of how these things reflect and contribute to their own salient sense of identity (e.g., Dumont, Yzerbyt, Wigboldus, & Gordijn, 2003;Häusser, Kattenstroth, van Dick, & Mojzisch, 2012;Haslam, Jetten, O'Brien, & Jacobs, 2004;Mackie, Worth, & Asuncion, 1990;Morton, Haslam, Postmes, & Ryan, 2006;Schmitt, Silvia, & Branscombe, 2000).…”
Section: A C C E P T E D Accepted Manuscriptmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Social identities may be more or less chronically salient but cues from the social context can heighten the salience of a social identity (Turner et al, 1994). The provision of comparative information about relevant outgroups (e.g., Haslam et al, 1995, Rabinovich et al, 2012, Tarrant and Butler, 2011 and questions that prime people to focus on a particular identity (e.g., Haslam et al, 1999) are examples of contextual cues that can make identity salient. When a particular social identity becomes salient, the norms of that identity provide guidance about how to think, feel, and act (Hornsey, 2008), especially when people are highly identified with that identity (Fielding et al, 2008, Terry et al, 1999, Ellemers et al, 1999.…”
Section: Social Identity and Political Identity Saliencementioning
confidence: 99%