2020
DOI: 10.1002/bdm.2156
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Collective decision making reduces metacognitive control and increases error rates, particularly for overconfident individuals

Abstract: This research aimed to investigate the changes in judgment accuracy, confidence, control thresholds, and decision outcomes when people act in two‐person groups (dyads) compared with acting individually. First, we used interacting dyads to determine the metacognitive and behavioral outcomes of collective decision making and compared them with those of individuals. Second, we examined whether these changes were related to the trait‐confidence and bias of individuals working together. Using a within‐person design… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
8
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 8 publications
(14 citation statements)
references
References 97 publications
3
8
0
Order By: Relevance
“…this finding is nevertheless in line with literature that has found no pair advantage when the task context consists of general knowledge-based tests involving discrete choice decisions [55,56]. This underscores the extent to which performance greatly depends on the task context.…”
Section: Individual Versus Collective Decision-making Outcomessupporting
confidence: 92%
“…this finding is nevertheless in line with literature that has found no pair advantage when the task context consists of general knowledge-based tests involving discrete choice decisions [55,56]. This underscores the extent to which performance greatly depends on the task context.…”
Section: Individual Versus Collective Decision-making Outcomessupporting
confidence: 92%
“…A statistically significant and reliably traceable main effect on confidence rather than on knowledge seems to mirror several psychological studies that examine the impact of cooperation on overconfidence in non-financial settings (Bang et al 2017;Blanchard et al 2020;Koriat 2015;Schuldt et al 2017). It should be noted that these studies stand in contrast to the findings of Trouche, Sander, and Mercier (2014) who report an increase in objective knowledge through group discussions.…”
Section: Overconfidence In Joint Financial Decision-makingsupporting
confidence: 57%
“…Generally, overconfident investors predict the outcomes of future endeavors to be more positive than they will be and overestimate the precision of the underlying information (Odean 2002). While most psychological studies agree with the notion of a potentially harmful bias (Blanchard, Jackson, and Kleitman 2020;Goodie 2005;Moore and Healy 2008), some studies indicate that overconfident individuals are more persuasive and are attributed to greater perceived competence and commitment in a group setting (Sah, Moore, and MacCoun 2013;Vullioud et al 2017). However, the critical question entails how joint decisionmaking influences financial overconfidence.…”
Section: Overconfidence In Financial Decision-makingmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The importance of examining strategically opting out of providing an answer to avoid errors (“giving up behaviour”) becomes particularly apparent when comparing research investigating metacognitive miscalibrations such as overconfidence and meta-ignorance ( Blanchard et al 2020 ; Buratti et al 2013 ; Händel et al 2020 ; Kleitman et al 2019 ; Moore and Healy 2008 ; Schlösser et al 2013 ). An individual who never gives up on any problem is bound to waste unnecessary time and cognitive resources.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%