2003
DOI: 10.1080/0014013031000061640
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Cognitive flexibility and adaptability to environmental changes in dynamic complex problem-solving tasks

Abstract: People who show good performance in dynamic complex problem-solving tasks can also make errors. Theories of human error fail to fully explain when and why good performers err. Some theories would predict that these errors are to some extent the consequence of the difficulties that people have in adapting to new and unexpected environmental conditions. However, such theories cannot explain why some new conditions lead to error, while others do not. There are also some theories that defend the notion that good p… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

5
168
0
15

Year Published

2008
2008
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8
2

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 312 publications
(201 citation statements)
references
References 21 publications
5
168
0
15
Order By: Relevance
“…On one hand, it may prevent inadequate changes in correct procedures in the face of misleading information, as reflected by a small number of Lossshift errors in this group. On the other hand, this tendency may be accompanied by less cognitive flexibility to recognize important cues demanding new behaviors, compromising them to react quickly to potential Bthreats^and to potentially commit perseverative mistakes more often (Cañas, Quesada, Antoli, & Fajardo, 2003;Norman & Shallice, 1986;Rougier, Noelle, Braver, Cohen, & O'Relly, 2005;Spiro, Coulson, Feltovich, & Andersib, 1988). The other way around might be the case for Low ET subjects who, being faster in reacting to cues demanding rule-based adaptive changes, are more prone to change ongoing correct actions following spurious information.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…On one hand, it may prevent inadequate changes in correct procedures in the face of misleading information, as reflected by a small number of Lossshift errors in this group. On the other hand, this tendency may be accompanied by less cognitive flexibility to recognize important cues demanding new behaviors, compromising them to react quickly to potential Bthreats^and to potentially commit perseverative mistakes more often (Cañas, Quesada, Antoli, & Fajardo, 2003;Norman & Shallice, 1986;Rougier, Noelle, Braver, Cohen, & O'Relly, 2005;Spiro, Coulson, Feltovich, & Andersib, 1988). The other way around might be the case for Low ET subjects who, being faster in reacting to cues demanding rule-based adaptive changes, are more prone to change ongoing correct actions following spurious information.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Their popularity and range make them ideal for studying the acquisition and transfer of skill-based knowledge in a variety of complex interactive environments (Campbell, 1988;Cañas, Quesada, Antoli, & Fajardo, 2003;Funke, 2001). A typical CDCT includes several inputs connected to several outputs via a complex causal structure or rule (See Figure 1).…”
Section: Complex Dynamic Control Tasks (Cdcts)mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Baer et al 2008;Bishop et al 2004;Kabat-zinn 1990). In this context, cognitive flexibility is defined as the ability to adapt cognitive strategies in response to new and unexpected conditions, and as intimately related to the (re-)investment of attention (Cañas et al 2003).…”
Section: Mindfulness Meditation Cognitive Flexibility and Mental Healthmentioning
confidence: 99%