2008
DOI: 10.1002/cpp.579
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Cognitive biases in depressed and non‐depressed referred youth

Abstract: This study examined cognitive vulnerability in both depressed and non-depressed referred youngsters. Formerly depressed (FD) children and adolescents (n = 16) were compared to a currently depressed (CD) group (n = 18) on a self-referent encoding and memory task imbedded in a mood induction paradigm. In order to test the specificity of the findings to depression, the results of the FD were further compared with those of a clinical but never depressed (ND) group (n = 39) diagnosed with anxiety and/or disruptive … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

1
17
0

Year Published

2010
2010
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 20 publications
(18 citation statements)
references
References 45 publications
1
17
0
Order By: Relevance
“…It may be that depressionrelated information processing biases or negative cognitive styles (Tibremont & Braet, 2004;Timbremont, Braet, Bosmans, & Van Vlierberghe, 2008) affected children's perceptions of their OCDrelated impairment. Or, children with comorbid depressive symptoms may have difficulty delineating the cause of impairment, resulting in inflated OCD ratings.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It may be that depressionrelated information processing biases or negative cognitive styles (Tibremont & Braet, 2004;Timbremont, Braet, Bosmans, & Van Vlierberghe, 2008) affected children's perceptions of their OCDrelated impairment. Or, children with comorbid depressive symptoms may have difficulty delineating the cause of impairment, resulting in inflated OCD ratings.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Numerous empirical studies have confirmed the presence of these negative biases in depressed adults (Butler & Mathews, 1983; Cane & Gotlib, 1985; Mogg, Bradbury, & Bradley, 2006; Norman, Miller, & Klee, 1983; Nunn, Mathews, & Trower, 1997) and youth (Dalgleish et al, 1997; Dineen & Hadwin, 2004; Eley et al, 2008; Gencoz, Voelz, Gencoz, Pettit, & Joiner, 2001; Neshat-Doost, Taghavi, Moradi, Yule, & Dalgleish, 1998; Reid, Salmon, & Lovibond, 2006; Timbremont, Braet, Bosmans, & Van Vlierberghe, 2008; Whitton, Larson, & Hauser, 2008). Adolescents with symptoms of depression are also prone to selective abstraction, or intent focus on the negative details in a situation to the exclusion of positive or neutral details (Weems, Berman, Silverman, & Saavedra 2001).…”
mentioning
confidence: 96%
“…Indeed, some research has suggested that inducing sad mood may differentiate between those with a vulnerability to depression and those without (Kelvin, Goodyer, Teasdale, & Brechin, 1999;Taylor & Ingram, 1999). Such an induction has been occasionally used throughout the literature (e.g., Timbremont & Braet, 2004;Timbremont et al, 2008). This relationship between induced sad mood and depressive self-referential processing is theoretically rooted in cognitive vulnerability to depression, rather than current symptomatology; as such, it was not employed in the current studies.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…Similarly, preferential recall for negative rather than positive words that were previously endorsed as describing the self is also commonly used to measure a negative memory bias (e.g., Bradley & Mathews, 1983;Gotlib et al, 2004). Some results suggest that differing levels of depressive symptoms may also impact on recall, endorsement, or reaction time (Derry & Kuiper, 1981;Kuiper & Derry, 1982;Timbremont & Braet, 2004;Timbremont, Braet, Bosmans, & Van Vlierberghe, 2008).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%