1975
DOI: 10.1037/h0076272
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Cognitive appraisals and transformations in delay behavior.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

17
228
2
11

Year Published

1984
1984
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 382 publications
(258 citation statements)
references
References 11 publications
17
228
2
11
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, Mischel and colleagues developed the classic delay task in which children are told that if they wait for the experimenter to return (a considerable period of time), they will receive a larger price (e.g., more food) than if they do not want to wait to consume (or receive) the prize (e.g., Mischel. 1974;Mischel & Baker, 1972;Shoda, Mischel, & Peake, 1990). Several findings from this work are noteworthy.…”
Section: Mischel's Delay Of Gratification Taskmentioning
confidence: 55%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…For example, Mischel and colleagues developed the classic delay task in which children are told that if they wait for the experimenter to return (a considerable period of time), they will receive a larger price (e.g., more food) than if they do not want to wait to consume (or receive) the prize (e.g., Mischel. 1974;Mischel & Baker, 1972;Shoda, Mischel, & Peake, 1990). Several findings from this work are noteworthy.…”
Section: Mischel's Delay Of Gratification Taskmentioning
confidence: 55%
“…Thus, emotion regulation is defined as the process of initiating, maintaining, modulating, or changing the occurrence, intensity, or duration of internal feeling states and emotion-related motivations and physiological processes, often in the service of accomplishing one's goals (Eisenberg & Morris, 2002). Mechanisms involved in emotion regulation include attentional processes (e.g., attention shifting and focusing), cognitive constructions or appraisals (e.g., Mischel & Baker, 1972;Mischel, 2000), and sometimes even overt behaviors such as inhibiting movement toward an evocative object (Rothbart & Derryberry, 1981).…”
Section: Emotion Regulation Versus Emotion-related Behavioral Regulationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…First, conceptually, our approach builds on prior work on delay of gratification in children and temporal (or delay) discounting, showing that healthy as well as drug-dependent adults prefer immediate smaller rewards compared with larger delayed rewards (45,46), but that cognitive strategies can influence and even reverse this preference (47). Whereas such studies have focused on what our choices reveal about our relative desires for immediate as opposed to delayed rewards, the present work focuses on how we can actively take control over the affective impulses that motivate these choices.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, studies based on the delay-of-gratification paradigm have shown that affect-rich access to the sensory properties of rewarding objects (e.g., the physical presence of an appetizing marshmallow) tends to promote impatience to obtain these objects for immediate gratification, and this at the cost of receiving even more rewarding objects at a later point in time (e.g., two marshmallows; see Mischel and Ebbesen 1970;Mischel, Ebbesen, and Zeiss 1972). In contrast, affect-poor representations of the same objects (e.g., thinking of the marshmallow as a cloud) tend to promote greater patience and ability to delay gratification (Mischel and Baker 1975; see also Shiv andFedhorikhin 1999 andRead andLeeuwen 1998 for related results). More recent studies show that the impatient tendencies triggered by affect-rich stimuli can even carry over to subsequent unrelated tasks.…”
Section: Stimulus Affect Promotes Impatiencementioning
confidence: 99%