1979
DOI: 10.1016/0001-6918(79)90026-x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Choice reaction performance following an error

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

18
220
4

Year Published

2005
2005
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 235 publications
(245 citation statements)
references
References 30 publications
18
220
4
Order By: Relevance
“…Participants showed post-error slowing when they failed to detect a nonthreatening face. To our knowledge, this is the first evidence of post-error adjustments in a visual-search task, where response latencies are much longer than what is typically found in studies using choice reaction time tasks (Laming, 1979;Rabbitt, 1966), Simon tasks (Rigoni, Wilquin, Brass, & Burle, 2013), or Stroop tasks (Suárez-Pellicioni, Núñez-Peña, & Colomé, 2013). This result supports the hypothesis that sequential control adjustments impact performance for a prolonged time period (Cheyne, Carriere, Solman, & Smilek, 2011).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 80%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Participants showed post-error slowing when they failed to detect a nonthreatening face. To our knowledge, this is the first evidence of post-error adjustments in a visual-search task, where response latencies are much longer than what is typically found in studies using choice reaction time tasks (Laming, 1979;Rabbitt, 1966), Simon tasks (Rigoni, Wilquin, Brass, & Burle, 2013), or Stroop tasks (Suárez-Pellicioni, Núñez-Peña, & Colomé, 2013). This result supports the hypothesis that sequential control adjustments impact performance for a prolonged time period (Cheyne, Carriere, Solman, & Smilek, 2011).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 80%
“…Post-error speeding is inconsistent with the idea that error reactivity necessarily leads to a more conservative response strategy (Laming, 1979), or with the idea that error reactivity only depends on an interference of error-monitoring on post-error processing (e.g., . Instead, the evidence of post-error speeding provided by the present study, together with other findings -such as the "affective privilege" of Reeck and Egner (2011) (i.e., the finding that task-irrelevant valent distractors interfere with task processing whereas taskirrelevant nonvalent distractors do not) -suggest that, in the presence of valent information, error reactivity is better characterized as a domain-specific process rather than as a domain-general effect.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It has been shown that controls make some behavioural adjustments when they have committed an error: they slow down their response speed on the next trial (post-error slowing; Rabbitt, 1966) and they improve their accuracy (Laming, 1979). With the current task we found no evidence of posterror slowing in any of the groups (see also Jonkman et al, 2007).…”
Section: Behavioural Datamentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The second explanation (i.e., a priori bias) is that people become negatively biased against the response option that was just executed in error (e.g., Laming, 1968Laming, , 1979bRabbitt & Rodgers, 1977). This implies that errors facilitate response alternations and hinder response repetitions, both with respect to response speed and probability of occurrence.…”
Section: Explanations For Post-error Slowingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…1 This question is just a valid as when it was first articulated by Rabbitt and Rodgers (1977) over 30 years ago. One answer to this question is that, after he has made an erroneous decision, a man slows down on his next decision-an empirical regularity known as post-error slowing (PES; Laming, 1968Laming, , 1979bLaming, , 1979aRabbitt, 1966Rabbitt, , 1979Rabbitt & Rodgers, 1977). However, this answer raises a new and more interesting question, namely, why does a man slow down after he makes an error?…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%