2021
DOI: 10.1037/pspa0000263
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Choice changes preferences, not merely reflects them: A meta-analysis of the artifact-free free-choice paradigm.

Abstract: One of the prominent, by now seminal, paradigms in the research tradition of cognitive dissonance (Festinger, 1957) is the free-choice paradigm developed by Brehm (1956) to measure choice-induced preference change. Some 50 years after Brehm introduced the paradigm, Chen and Risen (2010) published an influential critique arguing that what the paradigm measures is not necessarily a choice-induced preference change, but possibly an artifact of the choice revealing existing preferences. They showed that once the a… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

0
22
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
1
1

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 29 publications
(25 citation statements)
references
References 90 publications
0
22
0
Order By: Relevance
“…SoA is based on the observation that the difference in the subjective value ratings that people give to options in a choice pair is typically larger after the choice than before (i.e., the values "spread" apart). This effect is highly robust (e.g., Carlson & Russo, 2001;Chammat et al, 2017;Holyoak & Simon, 1999;Izuma et al, 2015;Izuma et al, 2010;Lee & Coricelli, 2020;Lee & Daunizeau, 2020Lee & Holyoak, 2021;Russo, Medvec, & Meloy, 1996;Salti et al, 2014;Sharot et al, 2012;Sharot, De Martino, & Dolan, 2009;Sharot, Velasquez, & Dolan, 2010;Voigt et al, 2019;Voigt, Murawski, & Bode, 2017; see Enisman, Shpitzer, & Kleiman, 2021 for a meta-analysis).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…SoA is based on the observation that the difference in the subjective value ratings that people give to options in a choice pair is typically larger after the choice than before (i.e., the values "spread" apart). This effect is highly robust (e.g., Carlson & Russo, 2001;Chammat et al, 2017;Holyoak & Simon, 1999;Izuma et al, 2015;Izuma et al, 2010;Lee & Coricelli, 2020;Lee & Daunizeau, 2020Lee & Holyoak, 2021;Russo, Medvec, & Meloy, 1996;Salti et al, 2014;Sharot et al, 2012;Sharot, De Martino, & Dolan, 2009;Sharot, Velasquez, & Dolan, 2010;Voigt et al, 2019;Voigt, Murawski, & Bode, 2017; see Enisman, Shpitzer, & Kleiman, 2021 for a meta-analysis).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Similar blind choice-induced preferences have been found in adults (e.g., Enisman et al 2021) and older children (Egan et al 2010). They are considered a critical test of cognitive dissonance theory as the fact that these preferences arise even when participants are choosing blindly ensures that their choice and later preference could not have resulted from any actual initial preference for the chosen object (Chen & Risen, 2010;Izuma & Murayama, 2013).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 58%
“…Together with our findings of the emergence of blind-choice induced preferences around 2 years of age, these findings raise the question of what underlies this developmental change from known choice-induced preferences in infants to blind choice-induced preferences in 2-year-olds. If young infants' preferences are indeed already influenced by cognitive dissonance at 10 to 20 months, why then would they not show these choice-induced preferences when their choices were blind, as adults and older children do (Enisman et al, 2021;Egan et al 2010)?…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations