2010
DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.02.005
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Children’s thinking about their own and others’ regret and relief

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

8
84
2

Year Published

2011
2011
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 79 publications
(101 citation statements)
references
References 15 publications
8
84
2
Order By: Relevance
“…In contrast, the tendency in both children and adults to think counterfactually when outcomes are negative has a clear preparative function (Beck and Crilly, 2009;Roese, 1997;Weisberg and Beck, 2010;White and Lehman, 2005) that is likely to be perceived by children with HFASD because of the obvious benefits of avoiding future negative situations. Our findings largely support this interpretation; the features of the downward stories were acknowledged more frequently by typically developing children than children with HFASD.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…In contrast, the tendency in both children and adults to think counterfactually when outcomes are negative has a clear preparative function (Beck and Crilly, 2009;Roese, 1997;Weisberg and Beck, 2010;White and Lehman, 2005) that is likely to be perceived by children with HFASD because of the obvious benefits of avoiding future negative situations. Our findings largely support this interpretation; the features of the downward stories were acknowledged more frequently by typically developing children than children with HFASD.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…Children were also familiarised with the 3-point smiley 219 faces scale. Each face was explained to them (with a description of how each of the faces 220 would reflect how much they liked a food) and their ability to correctly identify the expression 221 of each face's was verified in a procedure similar to Weisberg & Beck (2010). Here, each 222 child was asked to correctly identify which face represented "yucky", "yummy" or "just ok".…”
Section: Target Vegetables 174mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These results suggest that many 5-year-olds have the capacity to reflect on how their memory will be useful in the future and use this information to guide their behaviour in the present. As described earlier, this developmental trajectory is consistent with findings suggesting that children also begin to demonstrate a capacity for doubly embedded cognition around age five in other domains Weisberg & Beck, 2010. Nevertheless, it must be noted that 5-yearolds did not perform anywhere close to ceiling levels on my tasks requiring this capacity, and so future research may wish to examine the performances of children as they continue to move through the early schooling years.…”
Section: Five-year-oldssupporting
confidence: 86%
“…Interestingly, doubly embedded mental time travel in the opposite direction might be important in the experience of regret, in which one reflects on a past decision that could have resulted in a more appealing relative future (present). Again consistent with Chapter 4's results, the earliest evidence for regret may appear in 5-year-olds (Weisberg & Beck, 2010. Future research may want to check for correlations between future-directed information seeking and these other versions of doubly Singly embedded mental time travel Doubly embedded mental time travel embedded mental time travel, as well as other types of doubly embedded cognition more generally (e.g., second-order belief reasoning, which may also emerge around five years; .…”
Section: Chapter 4: Future-oriented Information Seekingsupporting
confidence: 85%