1998
DOI: 10.1016/s0885-2014(98)90022-3
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Children's recoding in memory for collaboration: A way of learning from others

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

11
56
0

Year Published

2002
2002
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
4
4

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 39 publications
(67 citation statements)
references
References 22 publications
11
56
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Thus, an intriguing dissociation was observed between item and source memory. Although item and source memory are sometimes affected in similar ways by encoding and test manipulations, this pattern is not always found (e.g., Foley & Ratner, 1998;Hicks & R. L. Marsh, 2001;Lindsay & Johnson, 1991). Within the context of source judgments, item memory is typically assessed indirectly by estimating recognition performance from the source accuracy scores.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Thus, an intriguing dissociation was observed between item and source memory. Although item and source memory are sometimes affected in similar ways by encoding and test manipulations, this pattern is not always found (e.g., Foley & Ratner, 1998;Hicks & R. L. Marsh, 2001;Lindsay & Johnson, 1991). Within the context of source judgments, item memory is typically assessed indirectly by estimating recognition performance from the source accuracy scores.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…At test, the cognitive operations (anticipations) that are retrieved for the partners' actions resemble those of self-performed actions (Foley & Ratner, 1998a;Foley et al, 1993a). Manipulations of the recoding process result in predictable effects on the presence and strength of the bias.…”
Section: Prospective Processingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Manipulations of the recoding process result in predictable effects on the presence and strength of the bias. When 4-year-olds were told to "think about how you would put my pieces on when it's my turn" in the collage task, the bias was exaggerated; when recoding was minimized by instructions to "think about how I would put my pieces on when it's my turn," the bias was eliminated altogether (italics added, Foley & Ratner, 1998a). Hence, the adoption of a first-person perspective in a collaborative task involving actions by self and another person can lead to an increase in source errors.…”
Section: Prospective Processingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In such tasks, children sometimes mistake an experimenter's actions as their own. Indeed, Foley and Ratner (1998) reported "I did it" bias in a source monitoring task, where 4-year-old children tended to claim that they placed pieces on collages that were actually placed by an experimenter (see also, Foley, Ratner, & Passalacqua, 1993). This study clearly showed that the internal source (children) would be more salient than the external source (an experimenter).…”
mentioning
confidence: 72%
“…Two sources of information are qualitatively different. Foley and Ratner (1998) reported "I did it" bias in a similar source monitoring task, which suggested that an internal source is more salient than an external source. Although the present study failed to detect the "I did it" bias probably due to that children performed quite well in the source monitoring task, the previous studies clearly showed that the internal source was more salient than the external source.…”
Section: Monitoring and Executive Functionmentioning
confidence: 99%