1997
DOI: 10.1080/0305724970260103
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Children's Moral Orientation: does the gender of dilemma character make a difference?

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2

Citation Types

2
8
0

Year Published

2002
2002
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 13 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 19 publications
2
8
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In contrast, 59% of the U.S. participants' annual family income was more than $50,000, considered middle socioeconomic status. One possible explanation for the Turkish group's high Justice scores may be that low socioeconomic status might promote higher concern with fairness, rights, and equality in some Turkish students, consistent with other researchers' findings (Beal, et al, 1997). However, socioeconomic status did not have a significant effect on P scores in this sample.…”
Section: Moral Orientationsupporting
confidence: 87%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In contrast, 59% of the U.S. participants' annual family income was more than $50,000, considered middle socioeconomic status. One possible explanation for the Turkish group's high Justice scores may be that low socioeconomic status might promote higher concern with fairness, rights, and equality in some Turkish students, consistent with other researchers' findings (Beal, et al, 1997). However, socioeconomic status did not have a significant effect on P scores in this sample.…”
Section: Moral Orientationsupporting
confidence: 87%
“…It has been argued the choice of moral orientation is linked to other variables, such as socioeconomic status and the type of dilemma discussed, not to sex. Beal, Garrod, Ruben, and Stewart (1997), for example, have argued that low socioeconomic status may promote a concern for rights. Walker, de Vries, and Trevethan (1987), on the other hand, have demonstrated that personal or relational dilemmas tend to be discussed in terms of care and response, whereas impersonal or nonrelational dilemmas tend to be reasoned in terms of justice and rights (regardless of the participant's sex).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The fact that women scored higher than did men on both measures of justice was unexpected, and adds to the conflicting literature on sex differences in moral orientation. Taken with the results of previous researchers (Beal, Garrod, Ruben, Stewart, & Dekle 1997;Friedman, Robinson, & Friedman, 1987) it seems evident that there is not a clear -male‖ (justice) or -female‖ (care) orientation as Gilligan (1982) proposed. Future researchers may wish to examine how gender identity rather than biological sex is associated with moral orientation.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 53%
“…However, a number of studies found no significant sex differences in moral orientation (Beal, Garrod, Ruben, Stewart, & Dekle, 1997;Friedman, Robinson, & Friedman, 1987;Maqsud, 1998). These results led to a general agreement among most researchers that there are different types of moral orientations (namely, care and justice); however, it is greatly debated as to whether or not these differences are due to sex.…”
Section: The Assessment Of Moral Orientationmentioning
confidence: 74%
“…Using the transcripts, firstly, responses across both the dilemmas were independently examined to identify moral judgements, and then they were coded for the orientational logic which they represented. The coding procedure used was adapted from Beal, Garrod, Ruben, Stewart and Dekle, (1997), Johnston (1985) and Ward (1988). Children's responses were classified as showing orientation to care/concern for others or an orientation to justice/rights.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%