1974
DOI: 10.1044/jshr.1703.367
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Children’s Discrimination of Rhyme

Abstract: A CVCC rhyming test, which had two slightly different types of auditory differentiation, was given to 273 kindergarten to third-grade children. A related rhyming test was also given to 62 of the kindergarten children. The rhyming category which required differentiation at the ends of words was more difficult than the category which required differentiation within the words. Correct responses increased with successive grade levels, but category differences remained. Sex differences were not significant. The rhy… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

0
3
0
1

Year Published

1981
1981
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 17 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
0
3
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, segments such as syllable-initial consonant clusters appear to be units in naturally occurring (MacKay, 1972) and experimentally induced (MacKay, 1978) speech errors. In addition, the findings of developmental studies (Kirtley, Bryant, Maclean, & Bradley, 1989;Knafle, 1974;Stanovich, Cunningham, & Cramer, 1984;Treiman, 1985aTreiman, , 1985bTreiman, , 1993Treiman, Zukowski, & Richmond-Welty, 1995) and metalinguistic studies (Cutler, Butterfield, & Williams, 1987;Treiman, 1983Treiman, , 1984Treiman, , 1986Treiman, Salasoo, Slowiaczek, & Pisoni, 1982) support the role of onset and rime units in spoken English. Onset and rime blendings also occur naturally in English; consider the origins of brunch (a meal served between breakfast and lunch).…”
mentioning
confidence: 83%
“…For example, segments such as syllable-initial consonant clusters appear to be units in naturally occurring (MacKay, 1972) and experimentally induced (MacKay, 1978) speech errors. In addition, the findings of developmental studies (Kirtley, Bryant, Maclean, & Bradley, 1989;Knafle, 1974;Stanovich, Cunningham, & Cramer, 1984;Treiman, 1985aTreiman, , 1985bTreiman, , 1993Treiman, Zukowski, & Richmond-Welty, 1995) and metalinguistic studies (Cutler, Butterfield, & Williams, 1987;Treiman, 1983Treiman, , 1984Treiman, , 1986Treiman, Salasoo, Slowiaczek, & Pisoni, 1982) support the role of onset and rime units in spoken English. Onset and rime blendings also occur naturally in English; consider the origins of brunch (a meal served between breakfast and lunch).…”
mentioning
confidence: 83%
“…One of the earliest and simplest phonological awareness skills, the ability to rhyme also begins to develop during the pre-school years (e.g., Adams, 1990;Wood & Terrell, 1998) and continues to develop during the early elementary school years (Knafle, 1974). Rime awareness (large grain) develops earlier than phoneme awareness (small grain) and predicts later phoneme awareness (e.g., Goswami, 2002).…”
Section: Phonological Awareness: Rhymingmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…As children grow older their ability to make judgments about small phonological segments improves. From a very early age they are able to isolate and detect relatively large units such as syllables, and they can recognize rhymes (Knafle, 1973, 1974; Lenel & Cantor, 1981; MacLean, Bryant, & Bradley, 1987). Rhymes involve units that can be called intrasyllabic (Treiman, 1985, 1987), and in terms of size, they are usually somewhere between a syllable and a phoneme; to recognize that cat and mat rhyme, one must detect at some level the common two-phoneme segment at .…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%