2023
DOI: 10.1038/s41598-023-34049-3
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Children’s affective involvement in early word learning

Abstract: The current study set out to examine the underlying physiological mechanisms of and the emotional response associated with word learning success in young 3-year-old predominantly white children. In particular, we examined whether children’s physiological arousal following a word learning task predicts their word learning success and whether successful learning in turn predicts children’s subsequent positive emotions. We presented children (n = 50) with a cross-situational word learning task and measured their … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

0
6
0

Year Published

2023
2023
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
2
2

Relationship

1
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 4 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 65 publications
0
6
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Thus, reflecting on one’s own uncertainty in the knowledge of word–object associations and using such judgements to strategically select uncertain objects to increase one’s knowledge might be more difficult for young children—at least relative to simpler perceptual or memory tasks. Indeed, recent work suggests that while children show more elevated body posture, indexing increased positive affect, after a familiar word-recognition task relative to a novel word-recognition task, there was no association between children’s performance in the novel word-recognition task and their posture [ 36 ]. In other words, individual differences in how successful children were in the word-recognition task did not explain differences in their positive affect after completing the task.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Thus, reflecting on one’s own uncertainty in the knowledge of word–object associations and using such judgements to strategically select uncertain objects to increase one’s knowledge might be more difficult for young children—at least relative to simpler perceptual or memory tasks. Indeed, recent work suggests that while children show more elevated body posture, indexing increased positive affect, after a familiar word-recognition task relative to a novel word-recognition task, there was no association between children’s performance in the novel word-recognition task and their posture [ 36 ]. In other words, individual differences in how successful children were in the word-recognition task did not explain differences in their positive affect after completing the task.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Beyond just the cues that occur, infants' experience of communication happens within a system that is constantly changing (see Thelen & Smith, 1994 for a review). Factors including infants' internal states and features of the environment vary at multiple timescales and influence the way that communicative input is encountered and processed (Mani & Ackermann, 2018;Outters et al, 2023;Pomper & Saffran, 2019). As one example, recent evidence suggests that the presence or absence of highly salient familiar objects may influence infants' word learning.…”
Section: Additional Influences On Infants' Experience and Processing ...mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…While everyday activities in the home (e.g., mealtime, playtime, book sharing) are one commonly recognized type of nonlinguistic context in which infant learning occurs (e.g., Kosie & Lew-Williams, 2024;Tamis-LeMonda et al, 2019) there is no clear-cut definition for what does and does not count as "context". Emotional states, spatial locations, social and political systems, communities and neighborhoods, and cultural values and beliefs are all examples of how context arises in infants' everyday experiences (Custode & Tamis-LeMonda, 2020;Outters et al, 2023;Rowe & Weisleder, 2020;Roy et al, 2015;Wu et al, 2021). Context influences infants' experience in multiple ways: certain words are likely to occur in specific locations within the home (e.g., "bubbles" in the bathroom at bathtime or "bye" next to the front door; Custode & Tamis-LeMonda, 2020;Roy et al, 2015) and caregivers' use of multimodal cues tends to be similar from day to day within an activity context but not across different contexts (Kosie & Lew-Williams, 2024).…”
Section: Additional Influences On Infants' Experience and Processing ...mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…First, positively valenced or moderately high arousal states in adults have been shown to support semantic memory (Gilet & Jallais, 2011), and negative (vs. neutral) moods are associated with worse recall of familiar and novel words (Guo et al, 2018;Miller et al, 2018). In toddlers, physiological arousal following episodes of word learning is associated with more accurate memory (Outters et al, 2023). These findings hint toward the possibility that the child's affective states may dynamically shape learning throughout the day.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…Children experience language in the context of activities, routines, feelings, and social interactions that change from one moment to the next. Affect is a particularly salient aspect of children's experiences that shapes (1) attention and memory (Guo et al, 2018;Miller et al, 2018;Outters et al, 2023), (2) communicative behavior (daSilva et al, 2021;Fields-Olivieri et al, 2020;Wass et al, 2022), and (3) contextual information (Andrews et al, 2009;). Yet, its natural fluctuations are nearly unexplored as a factor in language learning (Doan, 2010).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%