2004
DOI: 10.1002/acp.1012
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Children report suggested events even when interviewed in a non‐suggestive manner: what are its implications for credibility assessment?

Abstract: An important question in the legal context is how suggested statements about fictitious events develop over a course of various interviews. Sixty-seven first-graders were interviewed four times about one real and one fictitious event, applying various suggestive techniques. A fifth, nonsuggestive, interview was conducted by blind experts. Over the course of the interviews there was a considerable increase in assents to the fictitious events. Moreover, few significant differences in criteria-based content analy… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
26
1

Year Published

2009
2009
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 30 publications
(28 citation statements)
references
References 30 publications
1
26
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The generation of qualities characteristic of experienced events in representations of suggested events presumably can lead individuals to misattribute them to an experienced source. For instance, both children and adults who report being convinced about experiencing suggested events describe them with as much perceptual and contextual detail as they do experienced events (Blandon-Gitlin, Pezdek, Lindsay, & Hagen, 2009; Erdmann et al, 2004). This is not the case, however, for descriptions of suggested events for which individuals believe happened but deny any memory of experiencing.…”
Section: The Present Studymentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The generation of qualities characteristic of experienced events in representations of suggested events presumably can lead individuals to misattribute them to an experienced source. For instance, both children and adults who report being convinced about experiencing suggested events describe them with as much perceptual and contextual detail as they do experienced events (Blandon-Gitlin, Pezdek, Lindsay, & Hagen, 2009; Erdmann et al, 2004). This is not the case, however, for descriptions of suggested events for which individuals believe happened but deny any memory of experiencing.…”
Section: The Present Studymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Lindsay interpreted these findings as evidence that suggestibility effects are not entirely driven by social demands. Less direct warnings, such as cautions that the postevent information may have been inaccurate or that a few of the postevent details were wrong, have not reduced suggestibility in either adults (e.g., Christiaansen & Ochalek, 1983; Greene, Flynn, & Loftus, 1982) or children (e.g., Erdmann, Volbert, & Böhm, 2004), indicating that a direct warning likely is necessary to inhibit false reports due to demand characteristics.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Contrary to this misconception, we now know that after repeated suggestions over a long period of time children can give convincing, detailed accounts of events that never occurred, even when asked ''gold standard'' free recall questions (e.g., Erdmann, Volbert, & Bohm, 2004;Poole & Lindsay, 2001).…”
Section: Expert Opinion About Children's Testimonymentioning
confidence: 87%
“…In these kinds of studies adults typically have considerable difficulty evaluating the truthfulness of accounts from children (Ball & O'Callaghan, 2001;Block et al, 2012;Laimon & Poole, 2008;Leippe, Manion, & Romanczyk, 1992;Qin, Ogle, & Goodman, 2008) or from adults reporting childhood events (Ost, Vrij, Costall, & Bull, 2002;Schooler, Gerhard, & Loftus, 1986). Even the ability of experts to discern true from false reports is uniformly poor; in many instances professionals are no better than chance and some are reliably worse (Bond & DePaulo, 2006;Ekman, O'Sullivan, & Frank, 1999;Erdmann et al, 2004;Kassin, Meissner, & Norwick, 2005;Leichtman & Ceci, 1995;Vrij & Mann, 2001).…”
Section: Expert Opinion About Children's Testimonymentioning
confidence: 97%
“…For example, Saykaly (2009) observed that the presence of CBCA criteria decreases and altogether disappears at the third interrogation of a child. Erdmann, Volbert and Bohm (2004) noticed that in the fi fth interrogation of a child, only one CBCA criterion (quantity of details) diff erentiates true statements from false, and additionally some children begin to believe that they witnessed an event that never happened already after the fourth interrogation. Granhag, Stromwall and Landstrom (2006) observed a similar impact on RM in the case of fi ve consecutive inter-rogations.…”
Section: Themselves Note This Processesmentioning
confidence: 99%