1997
DOI: 10.3758/bf03211295
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Characterizing the intuitive representation in problem solving: Evidence from evaluating mathematical strategies

Abstract: Two experiments were conducted to investigate the nature of the intuitive problem representation used in evaluating mathematical strategies. The first experiment tested between two representations: a representation composed of principles and an integrated representation. Subjects judged the correctness of unseen math strategies based only on the answers they produced for a set of temperature mixture problems. The distance of the given answers from the correct answers and whether the answers violated one of the… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
3
0

Year Published

2001
2001
2009
2009

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 40 publications
0
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Moore and colleagues (Ahl, Moore, & Dixon, 1992; Dixon & Moore, 1996, 1997; Haines, Dixon, & Moore, 1996; Moore, Dixon, & Haines, 1991; Surber & Haines, 1987) have studied differences between analytical and intuitive strategies in tasks with clear-cut contrasts between the two. More research is needed, however, to understand the interplay of intuitive and analytic cognition in tasks involving both, as well as their relation in development (Schlottmann, 2001).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Moore and colleagues (Ahl, Moore, & Dixon, 1992; Dixon & Moore, 1996, 1997; Haines, Dixon, & Moore, 1996; Moore, Dixon, & Haines, 1991; Surber & Haines, 1987) have studied differences between analytical and intuitive strategies in tasks with clear-cut contrasts between the two. More research is needed, however, to understand the interplay of intuitive and analytic cognition in tasks involving both, as well as their relation in development (Schlottmann, 2001).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For each of the four basic arithmetic operations, we propose that people represent four principles. This set of principles is structurally analogous to principles that constitute the intuitive representation (e.g., Dixon & Moore, 1997;Dixon & Tuccillo, 2001) and would, therefore, be useful in a structure-mapping process. Because the basic operations are initially learned with positive integers, we focused on principles for this class of number.…”
Section: Principle Representation Of Mathematical Operationsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Moshman (2000) argues that the differences outlined between implicit heuristic reasoning and explicit analytical reasoning (e.g., Evans & Over, 1996;Stanovich & West, 2000) confound two orthogonal distinctions-namely, those between implicit and explicit processing and between heuristic and analytic processing. Evidence from studies of logical and mathematical reasoning (e.g., Braine & O'Brien, 1998;Dixon & Moore, 1997;Hawkins, Pea, Glick, & Scribner, 1984) shows that inferences consistent with normative principles are made automatically and that explicit reasoning can also involve the deliberate discovery and application of heuristics (e.g., Kuhn, 2000;Moshman, 1999). Moshman (2000) proposes that a multiple-systems framework should include four possible types of processing: implicit heuristic processing, implicit rule-based processing, explicit heuristic processing, and explicit rulebased processing.…”
Section: Appraisal Of Dual-process Theoriesmentioning
confidence: 99%