2005
DOI: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00332.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Change Blindness

Abstract: People often fail to notice large changes to visual scenes, a phenomenon now known as change blindness. The extent of change blindness in visual perception suggests limits on our capacity to encode, retain, and compare visual information from one glance to the next; our awareness of our visual surroundings is far more sparse than most people intuitively believe. These failures of awareness and the erroneous intuitions that often accompany them have both theoretical and practical ramifications. This article bri… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1

Citation Types

2
74
0

Year Published

2005
2005
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 246 publications
(78 citation statements)
references
References 15 publications
2
74
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Most researchers agree that scene representation is not picture-like, but instead is more schematic in nature (e.g., Henderson & Hollingworth, 2003;Hochberg, 1978, Intraub, 1997, Simons & Levin, 1997Simons & Rensink, 2005). The schematic character of representation has also been posited specifically in the case of transsaccadic memory (Carlson-Radvansky, 1999;Carlson-Radvansky & Irwin, 1995;Irwin, 1991Irwin, , 1992Verfaillie & De Graef, 2000;Verfaillie et al, 1994).…”
Section: Implications For Transsaccadic Memory and Scene Representationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Most researchers agree that scene representation is not picture-like, but instead is more schematic in nature (e.g., Henderson & Hollingworth, 2003;Hochberg, 1978, Intraub, 1997, Simons & Levin, 1997Simons & Rensink, 2005). The schematic character of representation has also been posited specifically in the case of transsaccadic memory (Carlson-Radvansky, 1999;Carlson-Radvansky & Irwin, 1995;Irwin, 1991Irwin, , 1992Verfaillie & De Graef, 2000;Verfaillie et al, 1994).…”
Section: Implications For Transsaccadic Memory and Scene Representationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Demonstrations of change blindness and inattentional blindness support this view, showing that without visual attention, significant events or changes can easily escape our awareness (Mack & Rock, 1998;O'Regan, Rensink, & Clark, 1999;Rensink, O'Regan, & Clark, 1997;Simons & Levin, 1997).…”
mentioning
confidence: 97%
“…Participants might have remembered studied pictures by maintaining visual representations (coding visual properties such as shape, color, orientation, texture, and so on), by maintaining conceptual representations of picture identity (i.e., scene gist, such as bedroom), or by maintaining verbal descriptions of picture content. It is possible that these latter types of nonvisual representation, and not visual memory, accounted for observations of high-capacity, robust picture memory (Chun, 2003;Simons, 1996;Simons & Levin, 1997).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…More recently, the phenomenon of change blindness has been used to argue that scene representations in memory are primarily schematic and gist-based, retaining little, if any, specific visual information (Becker & Pashler, 2002;Irwin & Andrews, 1996;O'Regan, 1992;O'Regan & Nöe, 2001; O'Regan, Rensink, & Clark, 1999;Rensink, 2000;Rensink, O'Regan, & Clark, 1997;Simons, 1996;Simons & Levin, 1997;Wolfe, 1999). In change blindness studies, a change is introduced into a scene during some form of visual disruption, such as a saccadic eye movement (e.g., Grimes, 1996;Henderson & Hollingworth, 1999, 2003b or brief interstimulus interval (ISI; e.g., Rensink et al, 1997).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%