2012
DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2012.07.001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Causal imprinting in causal structure learning

Abstract: Suppose one observes a correlation between two events, B and C, and infers that B causes C. Later one discovers that event A explains away the correlation between B and C. Normatively, one should now dismiss or weaken the belief that B causes C. Nonetheless, participants in the current study who observed a positive contingency between B and C followed by evidence that B and C were independent given A, persisted in believing that B causes C. The authors term this difficulty in revising initially learned causal … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
15
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 16 publications
(15 citation statements)
references
References 70 publications
0
15
0
Order By: Relevance
“…To model this, Fernbach and Sloman introduced a Local Computations (LC) model, which posits that people focus on "evidence for individual causal relations rather than evidence for fully specified causal structures" (Fernbach and Sloman, 2009, p. 680). By ignoring the possible influences of other causes, their model captures a strong empirical tendency for human learners to exhibit order effects and overconnect their causal hypotheses (also see Taylor and Ahn, 2012). Bramley et al (2017a) extended this finding, finding evidence suggesting that people consider local changes that modify their previously favored hypothesis.…”
Section: Learningmentioning
confidence: 89%
“…To model this, Fernbach and Sloman introduced a Local Computations (LC) model, which posits that people focus on "evidence for individual causal relations rather than evidence for fully specified causal structures" (Fernbach and Sloman, 2009, p. 680). By ignoring the possible influences of other causes, their model captures a strong empirical tendency for human learners to exhibit order effects and overconnect their causal hypotheses (also see Taylor and Ahn, 2012). Bramley et al (2017a) extended this finding, finding evidence suggesting that people consider local changes that modify their previously favored hypothesis.…”
Section: Learningmentioning
confidence: 89%
“…This effect obtains not only for beliefs participants have prior to entering the experiment but also those acquired as part of the experiment (e.g., Garcia-Retamero, Müller, Catena, & Maldonado, 2009, Exp. 1;Marsh & Ahn, 2006;Taylor & Ahn, 2012). In particular, for simple causal relations, a primacy effect is observed in the interpretation of contingency evidence that favors participants' initial hypotheses (Marsh & Ahn, 2006;Taylor & Ahn, 2012).…”
Section: Implausible Causesmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…Participants received $0.10 for their participation, a small reward consistent with past mTurk studies (Buhrmester, Kwang & Gosling, 2011;Brandt & Wetherell, 2012). Similar to studies by Gardner, Brown, and Boice (2012) and Taylor and Ahn (2012), only participants who had 85% or more of their previous mTurk assignments completed, as shown by the website statistics, were allowed to participate. Previous studies have shown that mTurk participants are highly motivated to complete the tasks, even when they were offered only a few cents for several minutes of work (Buhrmester et al, 2011;Mason & Suri, 2012).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%