The platform will undergo maintenance on Sep 14 at about 7:45 AM EST and will be unavailable for approximately 2 hours.
2023
DOI: 10.1111/cdev.13897
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Caregiver speech predicts the emergence of children's emotion vocabulary

Abstract: Learning about emotions is an important part of children's social and communicative development. How does children's emotion-related vocabulary emerge over development? How may emotion-related information in caregiver input support learning of emotion labels and other emotion-related words? This investigation examined language production and input among English-speaking toddlers (16-30 months) using two datasets: Wordbank (N = 5520; 36% female, 38% male, and 26% unknown gender; 1% Asian, 4% Black, 2% Hispanic,… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
3
0

Year Published

2023
2023
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
3
2
1

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 73 publications
0
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The idea that lower exposure to emotion words might be beneficial to the development of emotion recognition may appear counter-intuitive, particularly with regard to early developmental stages when children are in the process of acquiring the emotion lexicon and are learning to associate emotion words to their own sensations and those that are described to them or that they witness in others (Nencheva et al, 2023). Yet, the most direct evidence in support of the less-is-more hypothesis comes from a fascinating experiment by Peskin and Astington (2004), conducted with 5-year-old children.…”
Section: Is Less More?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The idea that lower exposure to emotion words might be beneficial to the development of emotion recognition may appear counter-intuitive, particularly with regard to early developmental stages when children are in the process of acquiring the emotion lexicon and are learning to associate emotion words to their own sensations and those that are described to them or that they witness in others (Nencheva et al, 2023). Yet, the most direct evidence in support of the less-is-more hypothesis comes from a fascinating experiment by Peskin and Astington (2004), conducted with 5-year-old children.…”
Section: Is Less More?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For instance, infants prefer emotionally-charged vs. neutral speech (Kitamura & Burnham, 1998;Panneton et al, 2006;Singh et al, 2002), actions (Zieber et al, 2014) and faces (LaBarbera et al, 1976;Reider et al, 2022). Second, emotions provide useful context that can help children construct complex meanings (Nencheva et al, 2023;Wu et al, 2021). Although we still have a very limited understanding of how affective displays interact with other communicative cues, there is some evidence that vocal emotion may benefit aspects of children's language development, such as recognizing words embedded in a speech stream (Singh, 2008).…”
Section: Emotionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Future studies can incorporate speech stimuli from male speakers to create an even more natural listening context and thoroughly investigate the impact of biological sex on infants' early processing of emotional voices. Additionally, further exploration is needed to understand how infants' neural sensitivity to prosodic emotion processing contributes to their subsequent emotional development, extraction of affective meaning, vocabulary acquisition, and other aspects of socio-linguistic development (Lindquist & Gendron, 2013;Morton & Trehub, 2003;Nencheva et al, 2023;Quam & Swingley, 2012).…”
Section: Limitations and Future Directionsmentioning
confidence: 99%