2014
DOI: 10.1558/lst.v1i2.101
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Bilingual children’s use of lexical strategies under narrative monologue and dialogue conditions

Abstract: Purpose This study examined lexical strategies produced by Spanish-English bilingual kindergartners across two different narrative elicitation tasks. Method Fifty participants (M = 67.24 months) produced narratives in English under monologue and dialogue conditions. Participants were placed in high and low language performance groups determined by results from a bilingual language screener. Per parent report, children were matched by age, current language use, and age of first exposure to English. Analyses e… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

0
5
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 39 publications
0
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Semantic errors occur when children have knowledge of the target's semantic category, thematic context of use, or functional and physical features (German & Newman, 2004; McGregor, 1997; Sheng & McGregor, 2010; Yan & Nicoladis, 2009). Semantic substitutions and circumlocutions are effective communicative strategies individuals use to compensate for lexical gaps or temporary retrieval difficulties (Dörnyei & Kormos, 1998; Dörnyei & Scott, 1997; Greene et al, 2012). The frequent production of semantic substitutions in the older children reflects more advanced cognitive and linguistic skills in these children.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Semantic errors occur when children have knowledge of the target's semantic category, thematic context of use, or functional and physical features (German & Newman, 2004; McGregor, 1997; Sheng & McGregor, 2010; Yan & Nicoladis, 2009). Semantic substitutions and circumlocutions are effective communicative strategies individuals use to compensate for lexical gaps or temporary retrieval difficulties (Dörnyei & Kormos, 1998; Dörnyei & Scott, 1997; Greene et al, 2012). The frequent production of semantic substitutions in the older children reflects more advanced cognitive and linguistic skills in these children.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…While “don't know” errors were uninformative as for the locus of word retrieval difficulties, the decrease of language-switches and the concurrent increase of “don't know” responses suggest changes at the pragmatic and/or cognitive level. Language-switches are yet another communicative strategy children use to demonstrate their linguistic knowledge (Greene et al, 2012). Frequent and successful language-switching from L1 to English has been observed in previous studies of Spanish-speaking children (Sheng et al, 2013; Sheng, Peña, Bedore & Fiestas, 2012).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…However, studies have yielded conflicting results as to whether bilingual children at risk for DLD differ from their typically developing peers in their language switching patterns and frequency of their switches into the non-target language. Some studies (e.g., Gutierrez-Clellen et al, 2009;Greene et al, 2014) have identified no quantitative or qualitative differences in switches out of the target language by Spanish/English bilingual children with low/impaired language compared to typically developing peers during narrative and conversation tasks. Greene et al (2013) found qualitative but not quantitative differences in language switching during a semantic task.…”
Section: Linguistic Predictors Of Language Controlmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Past work in children with typical language development has focused on language-specific skills as constraining children's ability to adjust their language choice to accommodate the current conversation partner or language context (e.g., Lanza, 1992;Genesee et al, 1995Genesee et al, , 1996Gawlitzek-Maiwald and Tracy, 1996;Nicoladis and Secco, 2000;Lanvers, 2001;Cantone and Mueller, 2005;Ribot and Hoff, 2014). Contributions of overall language ability to language control have been examined mostly in children with language impairment (e.g., Gutierrez-Clellen et al, 2009;Iluz-Cohen and Walters, 2012;Greene et al, 2013Greene et al, , 2014Mammolito, 2015). The current study demonstrated a relationship between overall language ability and language control in bilingual children across a broad spectrum of ability ranging from impaired to above-average.…”
Section: The Effect Of Language Ability On Language Controlmentioning
confidence: 99%