2020
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00968
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Cognitive and Linguistic Predictors of Language Control in Bilingual Children

Abstract: In order to communicate effectively with a variety of conversation partners and in a variety of settings, bilingual children must develop language control, the ability to control which language is used for production. Past work has focused on linguistic skills as the limiting factor in children's ability to control their language choice, while cognitive control has been the focus of adult models of language control. The current study examined the effects of both language ability and cognitive control on langua… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

0
1
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 11 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 93 publications
0
1
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Interestingly, cognate effects were not necessarily stronger in the duallanguage context. This is different from what we predicted based on e.g., Elston-Güttler et al (2005) and Gross and Kaushanskaya (2020). Differences between studies may be partly explained by several methodological differences, such as stimulus type and manipulation of language context.…”
Section: Language Contextcontrasting
confidence: 99%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…Interestingly, cognate effects were not necessarily stronger in the duallanguage context. This is different from what we predicted based on e.g., Elston-Güttler et al (2005) and Gross and Kaushanskaya (2020). Differences between studies may be partly explained by several methodological differences, such as stimulus type and manipulation of language context.…”
Section: Language Contextcontrasting
confidence: 99%
“…For example, duallanguage contexts are cognitively demanding for bilingual adults, as they involve multiple cognitive control processes, such as interference suppression and selective response inhibition (Green & Abutalebi, 2013; see e.g., Misra et al, 2012). Similarly, Gross and Kaushanskaya (2020) found effects of language context, modulated by several cognitive control abilities, in Spanish-English bilingual children between four and seven years old. Children with lower cognitive control abilities had more difficulty in maintaining the target language in interactions in dual-language contexts than in single-language contexts, producing more codeswitches and blends (see also Gross & Kaushanskaya, 2018).…”
Section: Language Contextmentioning
confidence: 98%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…For analysis, we divided participants into two groups: children with typical development; and children with LIs. Using converging evidence (e.g., standardized test scores and parent report) is best practice in identification of bilingual children with LI (Castilla-Earls et al, 2020; Sheng et al, 2012) and frequently a criteria threshold is used to operationalize LI in research with bilingual children (e.g., Gross & Kaushanskaya, 2020; Restrepo et al, 2013; Sheng et al, 2012). For the purpose of this research, we identified children with LI if they met two out of three of the following criteria: (a) received scores less than 85 (1 standard deviation from the mean) on the bilingual language index of the Bilingual English–Spanish Assessment (BESA; Peña et al, 2014); (b) had a previous diagnosis of LI from a speech–language pathologist; or (c) parents reported concern regarding their child’s language development on a demographic information form.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%