2016
DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2016.08.001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Automatic cueing of covert spatial attention by a novel agent in preschoolers and adults

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
5
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 51 publications
0
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…As in Samson et al (2010), the stimuli consisted of a picture showing a lateral view into a room with the left, back, and right walls visible; on test trials, zero, one, or two red dots were displayed in either one or two walls. During the familiarisation phase, participants saw images of a novel entity (based on the novel entity in Terrizzi & Beier, 2016) positioned in the centre of the room, with text representing the speech of the entity appearing above the room against the grey background.…”
Section: Apparatus and Stimulimentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…As in Samson et al (2010), the stimuli consisted of a picture showing a lateral view into a room with the left, back, and right walls visible; on test trials, zero, one, or two red dots were displayed in either one or two walls. During the familiarisation phase, participants saw images of a novel entity (based on the novel entity in Terrizzi & Beier, 2016) positioned in the centre of the room, with text representing the speech of the entity appearing above the room against the grey background.…”
Section: Apparatus and Stimulimentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the four experiments reported here, we implemented a novel version of the dot-perspective task that replaced the central figure with an unfamiliar object modelled after the stimuli from Terrizzi and Beier (2016). To minimise the risk that participants would spontaneously imbue this central figure with perspectival agency, we ensured that it lacked any characteristically agentic features.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…So a mindreading‐based explanation of the finding is equally possible, as Heyes herself notes (p. 267–268). Moreover, in a related paradigm, Terrizzi and Beier () have shown that “automatic” attention orienting is influenced by whether one believes (tacitly) that the target figure is an agent or not. If the figure interacts contingently with another agent prior to the orienting trials, attention‐shifts result; whereas if the figure goes through exactly the same sequence of movements and sounds, but not while interacting contingently with another agent, it does not.…”
Section: Mindreadingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Bayliss, Schuch & Tipper, 2010but see: Hietanen & Leppanen, 2003 -;Deaner, Shepherd, & Platt, 2007;Gobel, Tufft, & Richardson, 2017;Gregory & Jackson, 2019;Marotta, Lupiáñez, Martella, & Casagrande, 2012;Pavan, Dalmaso, Castelli, & Galfano, 2011). The literature suggests that gaze cuing is more likely to occur when the gazer has the necessary means to hold mental states (is intentional, Wiese, Wykowska, Zwickel, & Müller, 2012;Gobel et al, 2017;Terrizzi & Beier, 2016) and when the gaze cue is interpreted as part of an intentional gaze-shift (Hietanen, 2002). Although when two masks are worn by the same individual on front and back of the head, the gaze cuing effect fails to emerge even though participants can verbally report which of the two mask directions hid intentionality (Kingstone, Kachkovski, Vasilyev, Kuk, & Welsh, 2019).…”
Section: Intentions and Gaze Direction As Indicators Of Attentionmentioning
confidence: 99%