2019
DOI: 10.1123/jtpe.2018-0080
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Attentional Focus and Feedback Frequency Among First Graders in Physical Education

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
19
0
1

Year Published

2019
2019
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 14 publications
(21 citation statements)
references
References 21 publications
0
19
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The results showed best practices of auditory modalities such as verbal and sound cues by physical education teachers and physical education majors for children's fundamental movement skills. Moreover, Petranek, Bolter & Bell (2018) examined the type and frequency of instructions and feedback among younger children performing an overhead throw. 65 first grade students were participants in four experimental groups (external-high, external-low, internal-high, internal-low feedback).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The results showed best practices of auditory modalities such as verbal and sound cues by physical education teachers and physical education majors for children's fundamental movement skills. Moreover, Petranek, Bolter & Bell (2018) examined the type and frequency of instructions and feedback among younger children performing an overhead throw. 65 first grade students were participants in four experimental groups (external-high, external-low, internal-high, internal-low feedback).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, Becker and Smith (2013) only found a benefit of an external focus of attention in the complex task of riding a pedalo without handles, but not for riding the pedalo with handles. One study showed better performance during retention for the internal focus groups (Petranek et al, 2019). In the transfer tests, two studies showed better performance following practice with an external focus of attention (Hadler et al, 2014;Teixeira da Silva et al, 2017), whereas two studies (Emanuel et al, 2008;Petranek et al, 2019) showed better performance in the internal focus group.…”
Section: Implicit Vs Explicit (Practice-retention-transfer-realized Intervention)mentioning
confidence: 94%
“…In addition, five studies also included a control condition in which no additional instructions with a specific attentional focus were provided (Chow et al, 2014;Flores et al, 2015;Hadler et al, 2014;Perreault & French, 2016;Tse & van Ginneken, 2017). One study also included a comparison between a proximal (close to the person) and distal (directed towards a goal) external focus of attention (Flores et al, 2015), and two studies also manipulated the frequency of feedback for both attentional foci (high vs. low frequency) (Petranek et al, 2019;Wulf et al, 2010). Eight studies included a transfer test, which are described in (Table 3).…”
Section: Study Characteristics and Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For adults, a benefit to an external focus has been found in various motor balance tasks and sport skills (Shea & Wulf, 1999; Totsika & Wulf, 2003; Wulf, McNevin, & Shea, 2001; Wulf & Su, 2007; Zachry, Wulf, Mercer, & Bezodis, 2005). Among children and adolescents, results have been far more variable, with some studies showing an external focus benefit (Abdollahipour & Psotta, 2017; Coker, 2018; Perreault & French, 2015) while others showing an internal focus benefit (Emanuel, Jarus, & Bart, 2008; Petranek, Bolter, & Bell, 2018) or no differences between an internal and external focus (Palmer, Matsuyama, Irwin, Porter, & Robinson, 2017; Perreault & French, 2016).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In general, children develop automaticity more slowly and less completely than adults (Ruitenberg, Abrahamse, & Verwey, 2013), making automaticity difficult to achieve with unfamiliar tasks. Furthermore, the two published studies demonstrating an internal focus benefit for children used unfamiliar tasks by asking first graders to practice an overhand throw with a goal of proper form (Petranek et al., 2018) or asking young children (mean age = 9.4 years) to practice a dart throwing task (Emanuel et al., 2018). In contrast, children (mean age = 8.75 years) who were asked to perform a more familiar task practiced in physical education classes such as catching a tennis ball and adolescents (mean age = 12.7 years) who were asked to perform standing long jumps benefitted from an external focus (Abdollahipour and Psotta, 2017; Coker, 2018).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%