1997
DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01696.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Are There “Autistic‐like” Features in Congenitally Blind Children?

Abstract: Twenty-four congenitally blind children between 3 and 9 years of age were studied for the prevalence of "autistic-like" features, as assessed by teacher reports and by systematic observations of the children's behaviour. A comparison between the 15 blind children who had IQs over 70 and 10 sighted children group-matched for age and verbal ability revealed that a number of autistic-like features were more common in the blind. When the nine blind children who had IQs less than 70 were compared with nine group-ma… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

5
134
0
9

Year Published

2005
2005
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
5
4

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 226 publications
(148 citation statements)
references
References 34 publications
5
134
0
9
Order By: Relevance
“…Congenitally blind children are delayed in passing standard ToM tasks (21,22,(33)(34)(35)(36)(37). A major challenge to interpreting these data is disentangling whether blind children are delayed in acquiring ToM or whether blind children are confused by the experimental tasks.…”
Section: Reasoning About Beliefs Based On Hearing and Seeing In Eb Andmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Congenitally blind children are delayed in passing standard ToM tasks (21,22,(33)(34)(35)(36)(37). A major challenge to interpreting these data is disentangling whether blind children are delayed in acquiring ToM or whether blind children are confused by the experimental tasks.…”
Section: Reasoning About Beliefs Based On Hearing and Seeing In Eb Andmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Visual deprivation does, however, change the trajectory of ToM development. Blind children appear to be delayed in passing ToM tasks (21,22). It is therefore possible that, in congenitally blind adults, similar ToM performance is supported by different neural structures than in sighted adults.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Individual differences are found in the timetable with which the various achievements in mindreading may appear (Scholl & Leslie, 1999); these differences may be related to birth order (Dunn, Brown, Slomkowski, Tesla, & Youngblade, 1991;Perner, Ruffman, & Leekam, 1994) as well as to a rich exposure to elder children and adults (Lewis, Freeman, Kyriakidou, Maridaki-Kassotaki, & Berridge, 1996). Conversely, the maturation of mindreading may be slightly delayed in children who are congenitally blind (Brown, Hobson, & Stevenson, 1997) or deaf (Peterson & Siegal, 1995). This is unsurprising: the maturation of innate faculties often requires triggering or specific input from the environment or is guided, to different extents, by the external world (e.g., Bowlby, 1969;Chomsky, 1980;Fodor, 1981Fodor, , 1998Samuels, 2002;Scholl & Leslie, 1999).…”
Section: Step IIImentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Visual impairment is characteristically under-diagnosed in people with complex needs as the signs can be difficult to delineate. Diagnostic overshadowing where one condition may mask another is common in people with intellectual disabilities (Brown et al 1997;Cass 1998;Turner 1999).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%