2014
DOI: 10.1080/20445911.2014.944914
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Are bilingual advantages dependent upon specific tasks or specific bilingual experiences?

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

17
190
1

Year Published

2015
2015
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 218 publications
(218 citation statements)
references
References 70 publications
17
190
1
Order By: Relevance
“…without dense code-switching) give rise to enhanced executive control, and there is evidence to support this hypothesis from both children and adults of different ages. However, it should be noted that an increasing number of studies testing bilingual samples from similar dual-language contexts (in which codeswitching occurs to a lesser degree) have failed to find differences between bilinguals and controls (Paap & Greenberg, 2013;Paap, Johnson, & Sawi, 2014;. It remains to be seen whether some other factor(s) can explain the lack of a bilingual advantage in these cases.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 85%
“…without dense code-switching) give rise to enhanced executive control, and there is evidence to support this hypothesis from both children and adults of different ages. However, it should be noted that an increasing number of studies testing bilingual samples from similar dual-language contexts (in which codeswitching occurs to a lesser degree) have failed to find differences between bilinguals and controls (Paap & Greenberg, 2013;Paap, Johnson, & Sawi, 2014;. It remains to be seen whether some other factor(s) can explain the lack of a bilingual advantage in these cases.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 85%
“…Hence, BFs allow for evaluating the strength of evidence not only for the presence of an association, but explicitly also for the absence of a proposed association. Accordingly, using BFs has become increasingly popular in the area of cognitive enhancement (e.g., Antón et al 2014;Clark et al 2017;De Simoni and von Bastian 2017;Guye and von Bastian 2017;Kirk et al 2014;Paap et al 2014;Sprenger et al 2013;von Bastian et al 2017;von Bastian and Oberauer 2013).Based on previous findings, we expected positive associations of motivation (Brose et al 2012), a growth mindset (Jaeggi et al 2014), and conscientiousness (Studer-Luethi et al 2012) with change in training performance. Regarding neuroticism, our expectations were less specific, given that previous literature reported evidence for a negative association of neuroticism with mean training performance and transfer effects, but not with training gains (e.g., Studer-Luethi et al 2012, 2016.…”
mentioning
confidence: 75%
“…Hence, BFs allow for evaluating the strength of evidence not only for the presence of an association, but explicitly also for the absence of a proposed association. Accordingly, using BFs has become increasingly popular in the area of cognitive enhancement (e.g., Antón et al 2014;Clark et al 2017;De Simoni and von Bastian 2017;Guye and von Bastian 2017;Kirk et al 2014;Paap et al 2014;Sprenger et al 2013;von Bastian et al 2017;von Bastian and Oberauer 2013).…”
Section: The Present Studymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the 34 studies they review, Paap, Johnson, and Sawi (2014) observe that the bilingual advantage tends to be found in studies with small sample sizes and argue that the lack of a significant effect in large-sample studies coupled with significant effects in smallsample studies is worrisome (as it is unexpected given the properties of null hypothesis statistical testing). Donnelly et al (2015) also reveal a significant lab effect across studies, which they speculate could be due to socio-linguistic differences of subject pools or different data-preparation methods such as outlier removal.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%