2016
DOI: 10.1525/mp.2016.34.1.1
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Analysis, Performance, and Tension Perception of an Unmeasured Prelude for Harpsichord

Abstract: This study focuses on the relationships between music analysis, performance, and tension perception. Part 1 examines harpsichordists’ analyses and performances of an unmeasured prelude—a semi-improvisatory genre open to interpretive freedom. Twelve harpsichordists performed the Prélude non mesuré No. 7 by Louis Couperin on a harpsichord equipped with a MIDI console and submitted a formal analysis. Using a curve-fitting approach, we investigated the correspondence between analyzed segmentations and group-final … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
12
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(13 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
0
12
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Of the remaining 26% of papers containing a split point value (7 out of 27), four papers detailed music lessons ( i.e. , instrumental, music theory, or private/group lessons; Andrade, Vanzella, Andrade, & Schellenberg, 2017; Frey, Hautbois, Bootz, & Tijus, 2014; Russo, Thompson, & Cuddy, 2015; Sears, Caplin, & McAdams, 2014); one paper detailed formal music lessons (Weijkamp & Sadakata, 2017); another on instrumental playing experience (Hansen, Wallentin, & Vuust, 2013); and one in terms of years in an undergraduate degree in music (Goodchild, Gingras, & McAdams, 2016).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Of the remaining 26% of papers containing a split point value (7 out of 27), four papers detailed music lessons ( i.e. , instrumental, music theory, or private/group lessons; Andrade, Vanzella, Andrade, & Schellenberg, 2017; Frey, Hautbois, Bootz, & Tijus, 2014; Russo, Thompson, & Cuddy, 2015; Sears, Caplin, & McAdams, 2014); one paper detailed formal music lessons (Weijkamp & Sadakata, 2017); another on instrumental playing experience (Hansen, Wallentin, & Vuust, 2013); and one in terms of years in an undergraduate degree in music (Goodchild, Gingras, & McAdams, 2016).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In contrast, the greater number of years of MEC needed for affect-based tasks indicates that musicians with greater musical expertise were needed for more nuanced emotive responses. That is, greater musical knowledge was required to better respond to the task, for example, an experiment that required a scale rating of perceived tension based on implied harmony (Goodchild et al, 2016). Similarly, the greater increase in median number of years of MEC for combination-based tasks (9.5 years) may be due to the enhanced complexity of the task which needed more "experienced" participants.…”
Section: Experimental Taskmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In the work by Farbood and Price [12] the examined tones were static with fixed amounts of inharmonicity and the responses were given based on a pairwise comparison with relation to felt tension. However, since musically induced tension is a dynamic entity, it is very often measured through continuous real-time ratings in response to musical stimuli (e.g., [5]- [7], [15][16][17][18][19][20]). The presented study adopted this rating paradigm to obtain continuous responses regarding the brightness, roughness, mass and tension of synthetic tones with time-varying inharmonicity.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Wiggins et al (2010) suggest that music theory is a kind of folk psychology, and thus, might benefit from being more explicitly informed by music cognition. The music cognition literature supports the hypothesis that much of the way we perform music is informed by the way we perceive music (Farbood, 2012;Goodchild et al, 2016).…”
Section: Use Of Cognitively Plausible Features and Modelsmentioning
confidence: 93%