2016
DOI: 10.4324/9781315504254
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

An Introduction to Modern English Word-Formation

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1

Citation Types

0
55
0
8

Year Published

2017
2017
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 122 publications
(67 citation statements)
references
References 3 publications
0
55
0
8
Order By: Relevance
“…In particular, we ask whether children can exploit lexical flexibility: The systematic use of words to encode multiple, related meanings (Barner & Bale, 2002;Copestake & Briscoe, 1995;Pustejovsky, 1995). For example, many of the same English root morphemes can be used to label instruments, as nouns, and activities involving those instruments, as verbs (e.g., shovel, hammer, mix/mixer, wash/washer;Adams, 1973;Clark & Clark, 1979;Jespersen, 1942;Marchand, 1969; see Table 1 for other examples of lexical flexibility). The present studies explore young children's use of semantic generalizations about lexical flexibility to bypass observational learning: Upon learning one meaning of a new word via observation (e.g., that dax labels an activity), can children spontaneously infer another possible meaning of the word that follows a generalization (e.g., that dax can label the instrument itself)?…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In particular, we ask whether children can exploit lexical flexibility: The systematic use of words to encode multiple, related meanings (Barner & Bale, 2002;Copestake & Briscoe, 1995;Pustejovsky, 1995). For example, many of the same English root morphemes can be used to label instruments, as nouns, and activities involving those instruments, as verbs (e.g., shovel, hammer, mix/mixer, wash/washer;Adams, 1973;Clark & Clark, 1979;Jespersen, 1942;Marchand, 1969; see Table 1 for other examples of lexical flexibility). The present studies explore young children's use of semantic generalizations about lexical flexibility to bypass observational learning: Upon learning one meaning of a new word via observation (e.g., that dax labels an activity), can children spontaneously infer another possible meaning of the word that follows a generalization (e.g., that dax can label the instrument itself)?…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, irregular plurals may require the voicing of a final consonant, for example, before the addition of -s or -es (e.g., knife/knives). Phonologically, the -S plural morpheme is realised as /s/, /z/ or /iz/ depending on whether the base-form noun ends with a voiceless non-sibilant, voiced non-sibilant or vowel, or sibilant respectively (Adams, 1973;Christophersen & Sandved, 1969;Strang, 1969;Quirk et al, 1985).…”
Section: Descriptive Facts About the -S Plural Morphemementioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is important to note that the society needs the mass nomination at this stage of social development [1]. That is why semantic derivation, borrowings and abbreviations together with the traditional methods of word formationderivation and composition, are playing the important role in the vocabulary enrichment.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Restrictions on their usage are connected with the head word denotative meaning. Thus, the units RAM (random -access memory), ROM The usage of newly created constructions is quite popular among the brand names in sports, fashion [1]. For example, the well-known brand Adidas is the result of the fi rst and last name connection -Adi Dassler, who is the company founder.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%