2016
DOI: 10.1007/s00221-016-4567-y
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

An assessment of auditory-guided locomotion in an obstacle circumvention task

Abstract: This study investigated how effectively audition can be used to guide navigation around an obstacle. Ten blindfolded normally sighted participants navigated around a 0.6 × 2 m obstacle while producing self-generated mouth click sounds. Objective movement performance was measured using a Vicon motion capture system. Performance with full vision without generating sound was used as a baseline for comparison. The obstacle’s location was varied randomly from trial to trial: it was either straight ahead or 25 cm to… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
56
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

3
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 26 publications
(59 citation statements)
references
References 57 publications
3
56
0
Order By: Relevance
“…These results suggest that it might be possible to use echolocation in a range of distances longer than reported in several previous studies of echolocation with sighted participants in real settings (e.g. 55-65 cm [30], 62-130 cm [31] or less than 1.8 m [7,32]) or in laboratory settings with sighted and blind participants (up to 2 m [9,33]), when using larger obstacles in an otherwise reflection-free environment with low background noise. This was already explored by Schrnich et al [11] by measuring distance discrimination thresholds in echolocation up to 6.8 m using an AVR.…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 55%
“…These results suggest that it might be possible to use echolocation in a range of distances longer than reported in several previous studies of echolocation with sighted participants in real settings (e.g. 55-65 cm [30], 62-130 cm [31] or less than 1.8 m [7,32]) or in laboratory settings with sighted and blind participants (up to 2 m [9,33]), when using larger obstacles in an otherwise reflection-free environment with low background noise. This was already explored by Schrnich et al [11] by measuring distance discrimination thresholds in echolocation up to 6.8 m using an AVR.…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 55%
“…Two possible approaches to understanding the similarity between groups in the behavior of OC are i) OC during walking was a challenging task for both groups. OC requires that the individual detects the obstacle's position and edges, performs precise motor actions to move around it, and allows adequate space between the obstacle and the body at the point of moving past it to ensure safe navigation [21]. OC makes walking less automatic and more dependent on cortical control [22], causing a reduction in stability and increasing the risk of falling [11].…”
Section: Oc During Walking In People With Pdmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It was reported that during walking humans kept the same distance of approximately one-third of their step length away from both stationary and moving obstructions (Gérin- Lajoie et al 2005). During circumvention of a large obstacle located at the midline, and left or right to the midline of the walkway, the distance between a subject and the obstacle was found to bẽ 0.2 m (Kolarik et al 2016a(Kolarik et al , 2016b. The estimate of bodyscaled distance between a subject and an obstacle during circumvention of a large obstacle appears to develop with age: in children this distance was reported to be 0.4 m, which is twice larger than in adults (Hackney et al 2014).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%