2009
DOI: 10.1007/s11065-009-9120-3
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Aging and Spatial Navigation: What Do We Know and Where Do We Go?

Abstract: Spatial navigation is a complex cognitive skill that is necessary for everyday functioning in the environment. However, navigational skills are not typically measured in most test batteries assessing cognitive aging. The present paper reviews what we know about behavioral differences between older and younger adults in navigational skill and reviews the putative neural mechanisms that may underlie these behavioral differences. Empirical studies to date clearly identify navigation as an aspect of cognitive func… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

26
232
2
7

Year Published

2011
2011
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
8
1
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 306 publications
(279 citation statements)
references
References 71 publications
26
232
2
7
Order By: Relevance
“…Globally, the ability to navigate and orientate oneself decline with aging (for reviews, see Klencklen et al, 2012;Moffat, 2009). This lesser efficiency, which leads to more errors and longer execution times, is observed not only in situations where a predefined plan exists (Sanders and Schmitter-Edgecombe, 2012; but see Allain et al, 2005), but also in the absence of a predefined plan, e.g., in real-life or in unknown virtual environments such as a medical center or a supermarket (e.g., Head and Isom, 2010;Kirasic, 1991;Wilkniss et al, 1997;Zakzanis et al, 2009).…”
Section: Navigating and Orientatingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Globally, the ability to navigate and orientate oneself decline with aging (for reviews, see Klencklen et al, 2012;Moffat, 2009). This lesser efficiency, which leads to more errors and longer execution times, is observed not only in situations where a predefined plan exists (Sanders and Schmitter-Edgecombe, 2012; but see Allain et al, 2005), but also in the absence of a predefined plan, e.g., in real-life or in unknown virtual environments such as a medical center or a supermarket (e.g., Head and Isom, 2010;Kirasic, 1991;Wilkniss et al, 1997;Zakzanis et al, 2009).…”
Section: Navigating and Orientatingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Aging humans display progressive loss of spatial navigation skills (Moffat 2009) accentuated in those with even mild AD (Grossi et al 2007;Hodges 2006;Widmann et al 2011). Neurobiological and behavioral manifestations of cognitive decline relevant to humans are shared by several animal models (Gallagher and Nicolle 1993) including those genetically engineered to express attributes of human pathologies such AD (e.g., Adlard et al 2005;Jankowski et al 2005;Herring et al 2008).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Despite fundamental differences in the sensory information that is available and possibly integrated into the spatial representation of the part of the world that the subject is experiencing under real world versus virtual environments, researchers have generally considered that when subjects effectuate a viewpoint change in virtual reality (either by pushing arrow buttons on a keyboard or by manipulating a joystick), they actually imagine themselves moving, rather than imagining an array of objects and landmarks rotating around them [12]. Few experiments, however, have directly compared subject performance in order to determine whether the brain performs equivalently in these two differing conditions (see Moffat [17] for a review).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%