2020
DOI: 10.1017/s0142716420000247
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Age, frequency, and iconicity in early sign language acquisition: Evidence from the Israeli Sign Language MacArthur–Bates Communicative Developmental Inventory

Abstract: The current study described the development of the MacArthur–Bates Communicative Developmental Inventory (CDI) for Israeli Sign Language (ISL) and investigated the effects of age, sign iconicity, and sign frequency on lexical acquisition of bimodal-bilingual toddlers acquiring ISL. Previous findings bring inconclusive evidence on the role of sign iconicity (the relationship between form and meaning) and sign frequency (how often a word/sign is used in the language) on the acquisition of signs. The ISL-CDI cons… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2

Citation Types

0
7
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 68 publications
0
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…One of the first longitudinal studies on signing babies of deaf parents (aged 0;10 at the beginning to 1;6 years at the end) found that the babies’ sign lexicon did not consist predominantly of iconic signs ( Orlansky and Bonvillian, 1984 ). By contrast, recent studies with larger sample sizes of native-signing deaf children (mostly aged less than 3 years) have found that the more iconic the signs were, the more likely they were produced ( Caselli and Pyers, 2017 for American Sign Language; Novogrodsky and Meir, 2020 for Israeli Sign Language; Sümer et al, 2017 for Turkish Sign Language with a sample of deaf and hearing native signing children; and Thompson et al, 2012 for British Sign Language) and comprehended ( Thompson et al, 2012 for British Sign Language).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 85%
“…One of the first longitudinal studies on signing babies of deaf parents (aged 0;10 at the beginning to 1;6 years at the end) found that the babies’ sign lexicon did not consist predominantly of iconic signs ( Orlansky and Bonvillian, 1984 ). By contrast, recent studies with larger sample sizes of native-signing deaf children (mostly aged less than 3 years) have found that the more iconic the signs were, the more likely they were produced ( Caselli and Pyers, 2017 for American Sign Language; Novogrodsky and Meir, 2020 for Israeli Sign Language; Sümer et al, 2017 for Turkish Sign Language with a sample of deaf and hearing native signing children; and Thompson et al, 2012 for British Sign Language) and comprehended ( Thompson et al, 2012 for British Sign Language).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 85%
“…The common term used for this phenomenon in the literature is bimodal-bilingualism. Some use this term in a broad way (Lillo-Martin et al, 2014) and some in a more restricted way (Novogrodsky & Meir, 2020a). In the current study, we follow the broader definition of bimodal-bilingual.…”
Section: Language Interactionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the panorama of Israel, Novogrodsky and Meir (2020) research on the frequency, iconicity and age, in the literacy of bimodal-bilingual sign language students in sign language, implementing an ISL-CDI scanning tool, in sign language acquisition, in order to bring new findings to the frequency of language acquisition and the role of iconicity. Within the results obtained, outstanding effects are pointed out with regard to age, with correlations between the parents' sign language classification and the size of the sign language itself, supporting the validity by means of the ISL-CDI.…”
Section: Sign Language Teachingmentioning
confidence: 99%