1990
DOI: 10.1080/07340669008251528
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Age differences in the temporal continuity of gratings as a function of their spatial frequency

Abstract: This study compared young and elderly observers on the continuity of sinusoidal grating-pairs as a function of interstimulus interval (ISI) and spatial frequency (.5, 1.0, 2.0, 4.0, 8.0, and 12.0 c/deg). Consistent with prior research, the maximum ISI over which pattern continuity was maintained increased with spatial frequency. In addition, among older observers, grating continuity occurred at significantly longer ISI's at the two lowest spatial frequencies; no age difference was observed at the higher spatia… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

1
3
0

Year Published

1997
1997
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 9 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 31 publications
1
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The present findings offer little support for either the suggestion that age-related temporal losses are attributable to sensitivity declines (e.g., Kim & Mayer, 1994; Mayer et al, 1988; Zhang & Sturr, 1995), or that they are compensated at suprathreshold levels (Tulunay-Keesey et al, 1988). They are, however, consistent with prior studies reporting an age-related decline in the temporal resolution of high-contrast targets (e.g., Barrett et al, 1994; Buckingham et al, 1987; Hiller & Kline, 2001; Kline et al, 1990; Kline et al, 2001; Whitaker et al, 1992). When contrast was increased, counterphase flicker thresholds for both young and old observers at full luminance were elevated, but age differences were not reduced.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 91%
“…The present findings offer little support for either the suggestion that age-related temporal losses are attributable to sensitivity declines (e.g., Kim & Mayer, 1994; Mayer et al, 1988; Zhang & Sturr, 1995), or that they are compensated at suprathreshold levels (Tulunay-Keesey et al, 1988). They are, however, consistent with prior studies reporting an age-related decline in the temporal resolution of high-contrast targets (e.g., Barrett et al, 1994; Buckingham et al, 1987; Hiller & Kline, 2001; Kline et al, 1990; Kline et al, 2001; Whitaker et al, 1992). When contrast was increased, counterphase flicker thresholds for both young and old observers at full luminance were elevated, but age differences were not reduced.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 91%
“…Less obvious, but likely a result of declines in retinal illuminance and loss of magnocellular neurons, are reductions in temporal resolution. This is seen in elevated temporal contrast sensitivity thresholds (Tyler, 1989), as well as a prolonged time-course of backward masking (Walsh, Till, & Williams, 1978), visible persistence (Kline, Scialfa, Lyman, & Schieber, 1990), and the duration of after-images (Kline & Nestor, 1977). Essentially, the elderly will be disadvantaged in any experimental conditions where information is presented in rapid sequence.…”
Section: Sensory Aging and Its Impactmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Temporal slowing in the visual system as a result of healthy aging has been documented in a number of different paradigms that range from simple foveal flicker sensitivity (Kim & Mayer, 1994) and impulse response function estimation (Shinomori & Werner, 2003) to more complex stimuli such as temporal continuity judgments (Craig, Rhodes, Busey, Kewley-Port, & Humes, in press; Kline, Scialfa, Lyman, & Schieber, 1990) and second order motion (Habak & Faubert, 2000). Other senses have documented similar slowing, including the tactile (Gescheider, Valetutti, Padula, & Verrillo, 1992; Humes, Busey, Craig, & Kewley-Port, 2009) and auditory modalities (Wingfield, Poon, Lombardi, & Lowe, 1985).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%