2014
DOI: 10.1075/ml.9.3.03mcc
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Acquiring formulaic language

Abstract: In recent years, psycholinguistic studies have built support for the notion that formulaic language is more widespread and pervasive in adult sentence processing than previously assumed. These findings are mirrored in a number of developmental studies, suggesting that children's item-based units do not diminish, but persist into adulthood, in keeping with a number of approaches emerging from cognitive linguistics. In the present paper, we describe a simple, psychologically motivated computational model of lang… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
34
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
9
1

Relationship

1
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 76 publications
(34 citation statements)
references
References 29 publications
(45 reference statements)
0
34
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Thus, whereas the first two examples illustrated interactions between properties of a particular word and word order of an entire utterance, this example shows that semantic relationships between two words also affect word order. All of these examples of word and word order interdependence are broadly compatible with models of language production that represent production as activation of learned weights in a connectionist architecture; these representations arguably cross item and order memory (Chang et al, 2006;Dell and Chang, 2014;McCauley and Christiansen, 2014). In this view, language production models could serve as highly informative models of serial recall, especially when the models engage in sentence repetition (see Ueno et al, 2011 for word repetition and Fischer-Baum, 2018 for other potential commonalities in serial order representations).…”
Section: Potential Research Directions and Predictions For A Languagementioning
confidence: 60%
“…Thus, whereas the first two examples illustrated interactions between properties of a particular word and word order of an entire utterance, this example shows that semantic relationships between two words also affect word order. All of these examples of word and word order interdependence are broadly compatible with models of language production that represent production as activation of learned weights in a connectionist architecture; these representations arguably cross item and order memory (Chang et al, 2006;Dell and Chang, 2014;McCauley and Christiansen, 2014). In this view, language production models could serve as highly informative models of serial recall, especially when the models engage in sentence repetition (see Ueno et al, 2011 for word repetition and Fischer-Baum, 2018 for other potential commonalities in serial order representations).…”
Section: Potential Research Directions and Predictions For A Languagementioning
confidence: 60%
“…Tracking conditional probabilities may lead to the formation of chunks at later stages of learning, which then become elements themselves between which conditional probabilities may be tracked. In fact, recent models of language acquisition have demonstrated the feasibility of such a process (McCauley & Christiansen, 2014;Monaghan & Christiansen, 2010). Thinking of chunks as the outcome of statistical learning provides a direct connection with entrenchment: Throughout learning, frequently co-occurring elements and structures become more deeply entrenched, strengthening such representations.…”
Section: Implicit Learning Meets Statistical Learningmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…To date, MWUs have been investigated in studies on first language acquisition (Bannard and Matthews, 2008; Arnon and Clark, 2011; McCauley and Christiansen, 2014) as well as in work concerned with adult processing (Arnon and Snider, 2010; Arnon and Priva, 2014). Findings in the two areas suggest that both adults and children possess cognitive representations of MWUs.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%