2004
DOI: 10.1007/s00426-003-0154-5
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A short history of ideo-motor action

Abstract: The ideo-motor theory, which is currently receiving heightened interest in cognitive psychology, looks back on a long history. Essentially two historical roots can be presented. A British one, initiated by Laycock (1845) and Carpenter (1852), which was developed in order to explain ideo-motor phenomena by means of cerebral reflex actions. A second and older root is the German one by Herbart (1816, 1825), Lotze (1852), and Harless (1861), which considered the ideo-motor principle a fundamental mechanism of all … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

8
141
0
3

Year Published

2006
2006
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
5
4

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 261 publications
(152 citation statements)
references
References 23 publications
8
141
0
3
Order By: Relevance
“…Ideomotor theory, dating back to the 19th-century philosophers (e.g., Harleß, 1861;Herbart, 1825;James, 1890;see Pfister & Janczyk, 2012, or Stock & Stock, 2004, for historical notes), in fact ascribes to action effects a generative role: Their anticipation is what selects the corresponding bodily movement suited to produce the actual effect (see also Hommel, Müsseler, Aschersleben, & Prinz, 2001). Evidence for this has been provided with the response-effect compatibility (REC) paradigm (Kunde, 2001).…”
Section: Ideomotor Theory and Effect-based Action Controlmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Ideomotor theory, dating back to the 19th-century philosophers (e.g., Harleß, 1861;Herbart, 1825;James, 1890;see Pfister & Janczyk, 2012, or Stock & Stock, 2004, for historical notes), in fact ascribes to action effects a generative role: Their anticipation is what selects the corresponding bodily movement suited to produce the actual effect (see also Hommel, Müsseler, Aschersleben, & Prinz, 2001). Evidence for this has been provided with the response-effect compatibility (REC) paradigm (Kunde, 2001).…”
Section: Ideomotor Theory and Effect-based Action Controlmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Early formulations of this theory emphasized body-related, and especially proprioceptive, action effects as a means to address the corresponding motor patterns (Harleß, 1861;Herbart, 1825;James, 1890;cf. Pfister & Janczyk, 2012;Stock & Stock, 2004). In other words, the mental representations of upcoming proprioceptive changes are assumed to be linked to the specific actions that had caused them, and by this linkage, sensory anticipations gain the potential to activate the corresponding actions.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The focus of these early theoretical formulations on bodyrelated action effects becomes evident in different writings and can be succinctly summarized in the following definition of Bmovement ideas^(i.e., anticipations) as Bthe revival, through central excitation, of the sensations, visual, tactile, kinaesthetic, originally produced by the performance of the movement itself^ (Washburn, 1908, p. 280; see also Stock & Stock, 2004). In contrast to this focus on body-related action effects, empirical investigations of the ideomotor mechanism have mainly focused on action effects in the environment that are external to the body (e.g., Greenwald, 1970;Hommel, 1993;Kunde, 2001).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…An explanation of these findings has been derived from ideomotor theories of action control (e.g., Hommel, Müsseler, Aschersleben, & Prinz, 2001; for a historical overview see Stock & Stock, 2004). The crucial assumption of this approach is that motor actions are cognitively represented by their sensory effects; that is, by codes of the perceptual effects that contingently follow certain motor actions.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%