2014
DOI: 10.1037/xap000007
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A sequential sampling account of response bias and speed–accuracy tradeoffs in a conflict detection task.

Abstract: Signal Detection Theory (SDT; Green & Swets, 1966) is a popular tool for understanding decision making. However, it does not account for the time taken to make a decision, nor why response bias might change over time. Sequential sampling models provide a way of accounting for speed-accuracy trade-offs and response bias shifts. In this study, we test the validity of a sequential sampling model of conflict detection in a simulated air traffic control task by assessing whether two of its key parameters respond to… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
14
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1
1

Relationship

3
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(15 citation statements)
references
References 47 publications
0
14
0
Order By: Relevance
“…That nonexpert (novice) participants adopted a conflict bias in the current study without an explicit incentive to do so speaks to the face validity of the conflict detection task. That is, participants treat missing an aircraft conflict (i.e., possible passenger fatalities) as worse than making a false alarm (i.e., possibly increasing aircraft travel time; see Neal & Kwantes, 2009; Vuckovic et al, 2013, 2014). This finding supports the idea that people can make deliberate strategic adjustments to threshold bias to avoid failing to detect critical events (e.g., potential conflicts).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…That nonexpert (novice) participants adopted a conflict bias in the current study without an explicit incentive to do so speaks to the face validity of the conflict detection task. That is, participants treat missing an aircraft conflict (i.e., possible passenger fatalities) as worse than making a false alarm (i.e., possibly increasing aircraft travel time; see Neal & Kwantes, 2009; Vuckovic et al, 2013, 2014). This finding supports the idea that people can make deliberate strategic adjustments to threshold bias to avoid failing to detect critical events (e.g., potential conflicts).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One question of interest is whether PMDC is capable of fitting the performance data from our conflict detection task. Evidence accumulation models have proved useful for understanding conflict detection in the past (Neal & Kwantes, 2009; Vuckovic, Kwantes, Humphreys, & Neal, 2014; Vuckovic, Kwantes, & Neal, 2013), accounting for accuracy and RT, and providing sensible psychological interpretations of the effects of manipulations of bias and speed-accuracy instructions. However, previous models required task-specific inputs such as relative speed and angle of approach, and thus have limited generalizability beyond the specific scenarios on which they are trained.…”
Section: Testing Capacity and Cognitive Controlmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, a standard paradigm is to present stimuli with varying levels of support for one of multiple (e.g., two) alternatives (Erlick, 1961;Lee & Janke, 1964;Ratcliff, 2006;Ratcliff & Rouder, 1998;Swensson, 1972). Another benchmark involves the data that result from changes in the task instructions, such as emphasizing the speed or accuracy of the decision (Ratcliff & McKoon, 2008;Strayer & Kramer, 1994;Vickers, Burt, Smith, & Brown, 1985;Vickers & Smith, 1989;Vuckovic, Kwantes, Humphreys, & Neal, 2014;Wagenmakers, Ratcliff, Gomez, & McKoon, 2008;Wickelgren, 1977). Somewhat paradoxically, due to our consensus about experimental benchmarks, all serious theoretical contenders have been optimized to pass these benchmarks with ease, ultimately creating a theoretical stalemate until other experimental benchmarks are employed (Brown & Heathcote, 2008;Ratcliff, 1978;Ratcliff & Rouder, 1998;Tsetsos, Usher, & McClelland, 2011;Usher & McClelland, 2001).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Recent efforts have shown that accumulation models can account for behavior amidst additional task complexity. For example, this class of model has been applied to air traffic control conflict detection tasks (Loft, Bolland, Humphreys, & Neal, 2009;Vuckovic, Kwantes, Humphreys, & Neal, 2014), unmanned aerial vehicle simulation target detection tasks (Palada, Neal, Vuckovic, Martin, Samuels, & Heathcote, 2016), and medical image decision-making tasks (Trueblood et al, 2018). Using naturally varying stimuli (e.g., medical images) comprising covarying visual features, or more controlled stimuli (e.g., simulated targets) involving simpler decision rules, these prior studies have probed the generality of several different evidence accumulation models.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%