1962
DOI: 10.1007/bf02289645
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A recruitment theory of simple behavior

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

7
145
1

Year Published

1971
1971
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 189 publications
(153 citation statements)
references
References 16 publications
7
145
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Instead, a decision maker accumulates evidence in favor of each response and makes a decision as soon as one of the response accumulators reaches a criterial value. Although accumulator models have a long history in psychology (LaBerge, 1962;Vickers, 1970;Pike, 1973;Townsend & Ashby, 1983;Smith & Vickers, 1988;Smith & Van Zandt, 2000)-including in the study of recognition memory (Van Zandt, 2000)-they typically assume that the evidence accumulated within a trial is noisy. A ballistic accumulator model assumes no noise within a trialhence the term "ballistic".…”
Section: Theories Of Response Dynamicsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Instead, a decision maker accumulates evidence in favor of each response and makes a decision as soon as one of the response accumulators reaches a criterial value. Although accumulator models have a long history in psychology (LaBerge, 1962;Vickers, 1970;Pike, 1973;Townsend & Ashby, 1983;Smith & Vickers, 1988;Smith & Van Zandt, 2000)-including in the study of recognition memory (Van Zandt, 2000)-they typically assume that the evidence accumulated within a trial is noisy. A ballistic accumulator model assumes no noise within a trialhence the term "ballistic".…”
Section: Theories Of Response Dynamicsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The accuracy of all methods depended critically on the size of the approximating time step. The large (∼10 ms) step sizes often used by psychological researchers resulted in large and systematic errors in evaluating RT distributions.Over the past 40 years, models of response time (RT) for simple decision making have become very successful at capturing the details of observed data (Audley & Pike, 1965;Brown & Heathcote, 2005;Busemeyer & Townsend, 1993;Diederich, 1997;Heath, 1981;LaBerge, 1962;Lacouture and Marley, 1991;Laming, 1966;Link & Heath, 1975;Ratcliff, 1978; Ratcliff & Smith, 2004, Appendix;Ratcliff, Van Zandt, & McKoon, 1999;Smith, 1995;Vickers, 1970;Vickers & Lee, 2000). More recently, the same models have also become quite successful at explaining decision making at a neural level (Carpenter & Reddi, 2001;Cook & Maunsell, 2002;Glimcher 2003;Gold & Shadlen, 2001;Ratcliff, Cherian & Seagraves, 2003;Reddi & Carpenter, 2000;Roitman & Shadlen, 2002;Sato, Murthy, Thompson & Schall, 2001;Sato & Schall, 2003;Shadlen, Britten, Newsome & Movshon, 1996;Wang, 2002).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The question arises concerning the applicability of the model to other related situations, particularly those of easy discrimination, choice reaction, and signal detection. As envisaged by Laberge (1962), the model would be appropriate to different latency situations, including simple RT. In this respect, Pachella and Pew (1968) have obtained results supporting a generalized random walk model for simple RT.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Atkinson et Audley & Pike, 1965;Laberge, 1962) will be examined for its ability to account for latency features in difficult two-choice discrimination, and a preliminary comparison will be made with Experiment 1 was originally performed to assess the validity of different kinds of stochastic model for discrimination behavior (Pike, 1967). The accumulator model appeared to fit the data best, and the results from other work (e.g., Kellogg, 1931) also supported the model.…”
Section: The Accumulatormentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation