2011
DOI: 10.1080/02687038.2011.584690
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A multi-level approach to the analysis of narrative language in aphasia

Abstract: Background: Several studies have shown that traditional standardised aphasia tests may not be sensitive enough to adequately assess linguistic deficits and recovery patterns in persons with aphasia. As a result, both functional and structural methods for the analysis of connected language samples from people with aphasia have been devised (see Armstrong, 2000; Prins & Bastiaanse, 2004). Aims: The present article focuses on our attempt to provide a comprehensive, multi-level procedure for both structural and fu… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

5
157
1
4

Year Published

2014
2014
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
3

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 176 publications
(167 citation statements)
references
References 50 publications
5
157
1
4
Order By: Relevance
“…They can be fillers, repetitions, or revisions [34]. Phonological errors include phonological and phonetic paraphasias and neologisms [9]. A ratio of verbal errors was then calculated by dividing the total number of verbal errors by the total number of C-units in each discourse sample.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…They can be fillers, repetitions, or revisions [34]. Phonological errors include phonological and phonetic paraphasias and neologisms [9]. A ratio of verbal errors was then calculated by dividing the total number of verbal errors by the total number of C-units in each discourse sample.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, language is a highly complex cognitive function that rests on the interplay between different levels of processing. An interesting approach to solving the problem of narrative processing after TBI might rely on the possibility of performing narrative analysis that takes into account aspects of discourse at different levels [9]. For example, Marini et al [13] compared the narrative discourse of 14 non-aphasic speakers with severe TBI and 14 neurologically intact individuals.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Research studies reporting on this phenomenon are referenced in three recent reviews addressing discourse production in aphasia: one on describing the linguistic level of analysis [8], the second highlighting current theoretical and methodological challenges [9] and the third reporting on the quality of discourse information measures used in aphasia work [10]. On the whole, at the microstructure level, impaired narrative abilities have been linked to word level deficits [11], difficulties with verb semantics [12] and/or breakdown of sentence structure related to complex verb argument deficits [13]. In contrast, at the macrostructure level, PWA tend to produce coherent narratives regardless of word and surface structure deficits [1, 14].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Furthermore, significant correlations were observed between naming of nouns and the number of nouns produced in discourse, but not for noun type-token ratios nor between verb naming and number or diversity of verbs. For these reasons, it seems that picture naming of nouns and verbs is not always strongly predictive of discourse performance, and that discourse sampling may actually provide a more accurate measure of word retrieval deficits [11, 32, 33]. In relation to verb semantic complexity (e.g., instrumentality), recent evidence [12] confirms previous evidence from the monolingual aphasia literature [34] and findings reported earlier by Kambanaros and colleagues [29, 31, 32] for bilingual speakers, that PWA have no significant difficulty using semantically heavy verbs (e.g., fishing, building) when describing pictures or producing personal narratives.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%