1994
DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(94)90060-4
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A model of self-motion estimation within primate extrastriate visual cortex

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2

Citation Types

4
188
1

Year Published

1997
1997
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 229 publications
(196 citation statements)
references
References 114 publications
4
188
1
Order By: Relevance
“…This model provides a mathematically sound explanation for the removal of the rotational component of flow, but requires a complicated hardwired weight matrix. Beintema and van den Berg (1998) extend the template model of Perrone and Stone (1994) to incorporate eye-position gain fields as a solution to the eye rotation problem. While the STARS model also uses eye velocity gain fields, it does so at an earlier processing stage than does Beintema and van den Berg's model.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This model provides a mathematically sound explanation for the removal of the rotational component of flow, but requires a complicated hardwired weight matrix. Beintema and van den Berg (1998) extend the template model of Perrone and Stone (1994) to incorporate eye-position gain fields as a solution to the eye rotation problem. While the STARS model also uses eye velocity gain fields, it does so at an earlier processing stage than does Beintema and van den Berg's model.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The Lappe (1998) model explains a wider range of psychophysical data, but still requires embedding the Heeger and Jepson algorithm in the weight matrix. Perrone and Stone (1994) propose a model of the MT-MSTd network in which weights from MT cells encode templates of optic flow fields corresponding to different translational heading directions. The Perrone and Stone model can account for heading perception data in the presence of eye rotations, but Crowell (1997) identified certain situations under which the Perrone and Stone model makes incorrect predictions.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In this way MSTd could map out the relationship between the expansion focus and heading with relatively few neurons, each adjusting its focus preference according to the velocity of the eye. Similar models have been proposed by Perrone & Stone (1994) and by Warren (1995), but their models require separate heading maps for different combinations of eye direction and speed (rather than a smaller number of cells that are adjusted to account for eye movement).…”
Section: Visual Motion and Pursuitmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…If the flow field can be decomposed into these two contributions, then the focus of the expansion field corresponds to the direction of heading. This is not an easy computational problem, but several researchers have suggested various algorithms and explanations of how the nervous system might perform this task using retinal cues (Lounguet-Higgins & Prazdny 1980, Koenderink & van Doorn 1981, Rieger & Lawton 1985Heeger & Jepson 1990, Lappe & Rauschecker 1993, Perrone & Stone 1994. Royden et al (1992) recently showed that the eye movement itself generates a signal, presumably an efference copy of the pursuit command, that can be used to solve this problem.…”
Section: Visual Motion and Pursuitmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Indeed, this would be sensible if robust perception of self-motion ultimately relies on neural representations in which visual and vestibular cues are integrated. MSTd neurons have large visual receptive fields 18 , respond selectively to large-field optic flow stimuli 16,17 , and are thought to constitute a population code for representing heading from optic flow [19][20][21][22] . Electrical microstimulation of MSTd biases monkeys' reports of heading based on optic flow 23, 24 , indicating that MSTd carries visual signals used to judge heading.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%