2011
DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2011.1362
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A micro-geography of fear: learning to eavesdrop on alarm calls of neighbouring heterospecifics

Abstract: Many vertebrates eavesdrop on alarm calls of other species, which is a remarkable ability, given geographical variation in community composition and call diversity within and among species. We used micro-geographical variation in community composition to test whether individuals recognize heterospecific alarm calls by: (i) responding to acoustic features shared among alarm calls; (ii) having innate responses to particular heterospecific calls; or (iii) learning specific alarm calls. We found that superb fairy-… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

7
85
0
1

Year Published

2013
2013
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 71 publications
(94 citation statements)
references
References 64 publications
7
85
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…This is despite fairy-wrens typically not responding to unfamiliar heterospecific alarm calls with peak frequencies lower than 7 kHz [19], and never responding to 4 kHz synthetic calls with the base properties of superb fairy-wren alarms (experiment 2). Additional evidence for learned recognition by fairy-wrens includes responding to the aerial alarm calls of noisy miners, another honeyeater with low-frequency alarm calls that lack frequency modulation, only in areas where they live closely with noisy miners [22]. Furthermore, fairywrens usually scanned rather than fleeing in response to honeyeater calls played in reverse (ascending frequency rather than descending frequency), even though reversed calls are similar in most acoustic properties, including peak frequency [20].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…This is despite fairy-wrens typically not responding to unfamiliar heterospecific alarm calls with peak frequencies lower than 7 kHz [19], and never responding to 4 kHz synthetic calls with the base properties of superb fairy-wren alarms (experiment 2). Additional evidence for learned recognition by fairy-wrens includes responding to the aerial alarm calls of noisy miners, another honeyeater with low-frequency alarm calls that lack frequency modulation, only in areas where they live closely with noisy miners [22]. Furthermore, fairywrens usually scanned rather than fleeing in response to honeyeater calls played in reverse (ascending frequency rather than descending frequency), even though reversed calls are similar in most acoustic properties, including peak frequency [20].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We presented 108 calls to 12 fairy-wren groups in Canberra over six weeks in January and February 2011. We made a counterintuitive and therefore strong prediction based on the previous experiments using synthetic calls and our work on learned recognition in fairy-wrens [20,22]. We predicted that as peak frequency increased from a natural honeyeater 4 kHz, fairy-wren responses would first decline but then increase as peak frequencies approached the natural fairy-wren 9 kHz.…”
Section: (B) Playback Experimentsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…For example, treehoppers provision ants with a carbohydraterich excretion, but also produce vibrational signals to elicit costly defence towards predators [4]. A similar protection benefit has been suggested for the numerous birds and mammals that form mixed-species foraging mutualisms to reduce predation risk [7,[12][13][14]. By eavesdropping on each other's alarm and sentinel calls, group members can reduce their own investment in antipredator vigilance and thereby increase time available for foraging [14][15][16].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, despite divergence as a result of selection in other contexts, calls might include widely recognizable features [24], meaning calls are 'similar enough' across species for receivers to recognize them [25,26]. However, if divergence is too great for automatic recognition, repeated exposure could allow receivers to learn to associate the calls of other species with an appropriate alarm response [27,28]. Learning requires previous experience and, therefore, time to build up responses to the calls of another species [29], which may cause costly delays in alarm situations.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%