2013
DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2012.2539
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Alarming features: birds use specific acoustic properties to identify heterospecific alarm calls

Abstract: Vertebrates that eavesdrop on heterospecific alarm calls must distinguish alarms from sounds that can safely be ignored, but the mechanisms for identifying heterospecific alarm calls are poorly understood. While vertebrates learn to identify heterospecific alarms through experience, some can also respond to unfamiliar alarm calls that are acoustically similar to conspecific alarm calls. We used synthetic calls to test the role of specific acoustic properties in alarm call identification by superb fairy-wrens, … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

2
57
0

Year Published

2013
2013
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 59 publications
(60 citation statements)
references
References 40 publications
2
57
0
Order By: Relevance
“…As a result, any feature of a signal that speeds up recognition should be favoured [30]. In some cases, heterospecific alarm responses occur without previous exposure [2,24,26]. For example, the responses of superb fairy-wrens (Malurus cyaneus) to the alarm calls of allopatric species depend on the similarity of their calls [2].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…As a result, any feature of a signal that speeds up recognition should be favoured [30]. In some cases, heterospecific alarm responses occur without previous exposure [2,24,26]. For example, the responses of superb fairy-wrens (Malurus cyaneus) to the alarm calls of allopatric species depend on the similarity of their calls [2].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, despite divergence as a result of selection in other contexts, calls might include widely recognizable features [24], meaning calls are 'similar enough' across species for receivers to recognize them [25,26]. However, if divergence is too great for automatic recognition, repeated exposure could allow receivers to learn to associate the calls of other species with an appropriate alarm response [27,28].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We suggest that the solution to this puzzle is that conspecific receivers attend to specific acoustic features that are relatively invulnerable to degradation. In support of this idea, fairy-wrens attend primarily to peak frequency, the frequency at which amplitude is greatest, when deciding whether to flee (Fallow, Gardner, & Magrath, 2011;Fallow, Pitcher, & Magrath, 2013), and peak frequency is also resistant to degradation and little affected by reverberation (e.g. Wiley, 1991;Wiley & Richards, 1982).…”
Section: Conspecificmentioning
confidence: 79%
“…For example, fairy-wrens always flee to normal New Holland honeyeater, Phylidonyris novaehollandae, aerial alarm calls, but rarely fled when the calls were played backwards, which altered call structure but not peak frequency or audibility (Magrath et al, 2009b). Similarly, synthetic calls with honeyeater call structure but fairy-wren peak frequency provoke about 45% of birds to flee, again showing that acoustic structure of heterospecific calls, and not just peak frequency, affects response (Fallow et al, 2013). Other bird species have greater difficulty discriminating heterospecific than conspecific calls (Benney & Braaten, 2000;Lohr et al, 2003).…”
Section: Conspecificmentioning
confidence: 89%
See 1 more Smart Citation