1996
DOI: 10.2466/pms.1996.82.1.23
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A Cross-Modal Aftereffect: Auditory Displacement following Adaptation to Visual Motion

Abstract: It has been shown earlier that the perceived location of static sound-sources can be displaced (a) during visual motion and (b) following auditory motion. Here we combine these phenomena. The subject adapted to the horizontal visual motion of a surrounding drum, then (with the lights off) localized static sound-sources by setting the direction of a pointer. Adapting motion was clockwise or counterclockwise: the difference between each subject's settings following the opposite directions of adaptation showed sm… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
11
0

Year Published

1996
1996
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 24 publications
(14 citation statements)
references
References 8 publications
3
11
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The demonstration of an auditory aftereffect following adaptation to visual motion is consistent with previous findings from Kitagawa and Ichihara (2002) and Ehrenstein and Reinhardt-Rutland (1996). Kitagawa and Ichihara examined the effect of adapting to auditory and visual stimuli, moving in depth, on stationary test stimuli.…”
Section: Methodssupporting
confidence: 86%
“…The demonstration of an auditory aftereffect following adaptation to visual motion is consistent with previous findings from Kitagawa and Ichihara (2002) and Ehrenstein and Reinhardt-Rutland (1996). Kitagawa and Ichihara examined the effect of adapting to auditory and visual stimuli, moving in depth, on stationary test stimuli.…”
Section: Methodssupporting
confidence: 86%
“…In humans, most research work on multisensory integration is still at the stage of demonstrating the phenomenon and understanding the operative factors at perceptual and behavioral levels (for review, see O'Hare, 1991;Welch & Warren, 1986). Most frequently investigated are the aspects related to intersensory bias, that is, the capacity of a sensory system to modify-enhance, degrade-the perception from another system (e.g., Bernstein, 1970;Hershenson, 1962;Hubbard, 1966;McGurk & MacDonald, 1976;O'Leary & Rhodes, 1984;Sekuler, Sekuler, & Lau, 1997) and the relationships between this bias and multimodal spatial processing (how the sensory signals compete with one another according to the relative spatial position of the unimodal cues) (Bertelson & Radeau, 1981;Ehrenstein & Reinhardt-Rutland, 1996;Fisher & Pylyshyn, 1994;Frens, Vanopstal, & Vanderwilligen, 1995;Held, 1955;Pick, Warren, & Hay, 1969;Shelton & Searle, 1980;Stein, Meredith, Huneycutt, & McDade, 1989;Warren, McCarthy, & Welch, 1983) or the relationships between intersensory bias and the subject's attentional focus (Bernstein, 1970;Miller, 1982Miller, , 1986Spence & Driver, 1997). In these investigations, the tasks have been most often to detect, localize, and/or react to stimuli containing cues from one or more sensory modalities.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In everyday life, we perceive a real object by hearing as well as seeing and touching. Several studies have shown that self-motion information systematically biases auditory spatial perception, such as the localisation or lateralisation of a sound (Clark and Graybiel 1949;Cullen et al 1992;Ehrenstein and Reinhardt-Rutland 1996;Thurlow and Kerr 1970). However, in no study has the effect of self-motion information on temporal aspects such as perceived temporal order of auditory events been investigated.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%