2015
DOI: 10.2139/ssrn.2709837
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A Bridge from Monty Hall to the (Anti-)Hot Hand: Restricted Choice, Selection Bias, and Empirical Practice

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

0
18
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
2
1

Relationship

1
2

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 3 publications
(18 citation statements)
references
References 53 publications
0
18
0
Order By: Relevance
“…21 While GVT's analysis of conditional probabilities provides their only measure of the magnitude of the hot hand, they also analyze the number of runs, serial correlation, and variation of shooting percentage in 4-shot windows. Miller and Sanjurjo (2014) show that the runs and serial correlation tests, along with the conditional probability test for k = 1, all amount to roughly the same test, and moreover, that they are not sufficiently powered to identify hot hand shooting. The reason why is due to measurement error: the act of hitting a single shot is only a weak signal of a change in a player's underlying probability of success, which leads to an attenuation bias in the estimate of the increase in the probability of success associated with entering the hot state (see Appendix B and Stone (2012)'s work on measurement error when estimating autocorrelation in ability).…”
Section: Gvt's Analysismentioning
confidence: 97%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…21 While GVT's analysis of conditional probabilities provides their only measure of the magnitude of the hot hand, they also analyze the number of runs, serial correlation, and variation of shooting percentage in 4-shot windows. Miller and Sanjurjo (2014) show that the runs and serial correlation tests, along with the conditional probability test for k = 1, all amount to roughly the same test, and moreover, that they are not sufficiently powered to identify hot hand shooting. The reason why is due to measurement error: the act of hitting a single shot is only a weak signal of a change in a player's underlying probability of success, which leads to an attenuation bias in the estimate of the increase in the probability of success associated with entering the hot state (see Appendix B and Stone (2012)'s work on measurement error when estimating autocorrelation in ability).…”
Section: Gvt's Analysismentioning
confidence: 97%
“…Outline of Proof : In the proof contained in Appendix A we begin by showing that the conditional expectation E[P k (X)|I k (X) = ∅] is equal to the conditional probability P(X τ = 1|I k (X) = ∅), (Friedman 1998;Nalebuff 1987;Selvin 1975;Vos Savant 1990). See Miller and Sanjurjo (2015a) for more details on the connection between the selection bias, the Monty Hall problem, and other conditional probability puzzles. 11 In particular, Miller and Sanjurjo (2016) show that the bias introduced here, in conjunction with a quasi-Bayesian model of decision making under sample size neglect (Benjamin, Rabin, and Raymond 2014;Griffin and Tversky 1992;Kahneman and Tversky 1972), provides a novel structural candidate explanation for the persistence of gambler's fallacy beliefs.…”
Section: The Streak Selection Biasmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Much is at stake in evaluating these competing explanations. If the main cause is improper Bayesian updating following restricted choice, then the Monty Hall problem is a decision‐theoretic manifestation of longstanding statistical puzzles in education, psychology, and sports science (Miller and Sanjurjo ) and in medical decision‐making (Cox et al ). If it is a more general failure of hypothetical thinking, then we get new insight into economically important phenomena such as the “winner's curse” in common value auctions and a variety of other “cursed equilibria” (Esponda and Vespa ; Eyster and Rabin ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%