2014
DOI: 10.1167/14.3.8
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

More target features in visual working memory leads to poorer search guidance: Evidence from contralateral delay activity

Abstract: The visual-search literature has assumed that the top-down target representation used to guide search resides in visual working memory (VWM). We directly tested this assumption using contralateral delay activity (CDA) to estimate the VWM load imposed by the target representation. In Experiment 1, observers previewed four photorealistic objects and were cued to remember the two objects appearing to the left or right of central fixation; Experiment 2 was identical except that observers previewed two photorealist… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

6
36
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 14 publications
(42 citation statements)
references
References 48 publications
6
36
0
Order By: Relevance
“…It was shown that when a new target is presented it is being held in VWM but then gradually moves to LTM. This hand-off between VWM and LTM leads to better search efficiency, so that as the CDA amplitude decreased (indicating less involvement of VWM) search RT improved (Carlisle et al, 2011; Schmidt et al, 2014; Woodman and Arita, 2011). The hand-off between VWM and LTM did not depend on the target complexity level, but when the target was associated with a high reward, VWM continued to represent the target, indicating a more controlled search in high reward conditions (Gunseli et al, 2014a,b; Reinhart and Woodman, 2014).…”
Section: Using the Cda To Study Vwmmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It was shown that when a new target is presented it is being held in VWM but then gradually moves to LTM. This hand-off between VWM and LTM leads to better search efficiency, so that as the CDA amplitude decreased (indicating less involvement of VWM) search RT improved (Carlisle et al, 2011; Schmidt et al, 2014; Woodman and Arita, 2011). The hand-off between VWM and LTM did not depend on the target complexity level, but when the target was associated with a high reward, VWM continued to represent the target, indicating a more controlled search in high reward conditions (Gunseli et al, 2014a,b; Reinhart and Woodman, 2014).…”
Section: Using the Cda To Study Vwmmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This relationship is clearest in the case of the large and growing literature on feature templates that are maintained in VWM and used to guide behavior, and the brain mechanisms engaged in this maintenance . These templates are thought to be neural representations of goal states that can be used to mediate a variety of tasks, with change detection and visual search being among the most commonly studied . In the case of search, a VWM representation of a search target is compared to incoming visual information for the purpose of creating a target map.…”
Section: Priority Maps and Vwmmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Complex actions certainly emerge from highly dynamic systems, but even seemingly static target goals likely emerge from the dynamic weighting of features allowing expected targets to be discriminated from expected distractors. In this sense, the target of a search task is not a static entity or “template,” but rather a goal state that emerges over time …”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The logic underlying this prediction is straightforward. To the extent that CCFs are the important features in the representation of a category, more CCFs mean a better and more specific category representation (see also Schmidt, MacNamara, Proudfit, & Zelinsky, 2014). A target category having a larger number of CCFs would therefore be represented with a higher degree of specificity and, consequently, fixated more efficiently than a target having a sparser “template” (Schmidt & Zelinsky, 2009).…”
Section: Model Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%