2012
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00272
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Embodied Space in Early Blind Individuals

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

1
14
0

Year Published

2013
2013
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 18 publications
(15 citation statements)
references
References 39 publications
1
14
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Specifically, the way blind people experience spatial relationships sensorially may cause their temporal concepts to be constructed differently than their sighted counterparts', regardless of reading experience. Several experiments have shown that the mental organization of nonvisual spatial frames of reference (FoR) in early blind (who lost their sight before age ~3) is qualitatively different compared to sighted people (Crollen & Collignon, 2012). Studies of tactile stimulus localization (Röder, Rösler & Spence, 2004) and multisensory control of action (Collignon, Charbonneau, Lassonde & Lepore, 2009;Röder, Kusmierek, Spence & Schicke, 2007) show that whereas sighted people tend to rely on an external spatial FoR (i.e., locations are represented within a framework external to the body), the early blind preferentially use an anatomical FoR (i.e., locations are represented with respect to the position of one's body and the position of one's limbs), to represent spatial relationships.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Specifically, the way blind people experience spatial relationships sensorially may cause their temporal concepts to be constructed differently than their sighted counterparts', regardless of reading experience. Several experiments have shown that the mental organization of nonvisual spatial frames of reference (FoR) in early blind (who lost their sight before age ~3) is qualitatively different compared to sighted people (Crollen & Collignon, 2012). Studies of tactile stimulus localization (Röder, Rösler & Spence, 2004) and multisensory control of action (Collignon, Charbonneau, Lassonde & Lepore, 2009;Röder, Kusmierek, Spence & Schicke, 2007) show that whereas sighted people tend to rely on an external spatial FoR (i.e., locations are represented within a framework external to the body), the early blind preferentially use an anatomical FoR (i.e., locations are represented with respect to the position of one's body and the position of one's limbs), to represent spatial relationships.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This could explain, partially at least, why congenitally blind people show level of performance in object localization and manipulation comparable to sighted agents 13 16 . However, some studies argued and provided evidence that visual experience may exert a dominant role in the representation of space, even affecting the auditory spatial maps, which might be involved in action planning and control 17 , 18 . These studies would seem to support the hypothesis that congenitally blind people represent their space differently from sighted agents 18 21 .…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…They perform even better than sighted in various numerosity estimation tasks (Castronovo & Delvenne, 2013;Castronovo & Seron, 2007b;Ferrand, Riggs, & Castronovo, 2010) and in some calculation experiments involving addition and multiplication operations (Dormal, Crollen, Baumans, Lepore, & Collignon, 2016). Vision is therefore not mandatory for the emergence of numerical-spatial interactions, even though visual experience affects the nature of this relation (Crollen & Collignon, 2012).…”
Section: Interactions Between Number and Space In Individuals With Blmentioning
confidence: 99%