2014
DOI: 10.1111/desc.12177
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Young children ‘solve for x’ using the Approximate Number System

Abstract: The Approximate Number System (ANS) supports basic arithmetic computation in early childhood, but it is unclear whether the ANS also supports the more complex computations introduced later in formal education. ‘Solving for x’ in addend-unknown problems is notoriously difficult for children, who often struggle with these types of problems well into high school. Here we asked whether 4–6-year-old children could solve for an unknown addend using the ANS. We presented problems either symbolically, using Arabic num… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

5
34
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 19 publications
(39 citation statements)
references
References 45 publications
(66 reference statements)
5
34
0
Order By: Relevance
“…For instance, Gelman and Gallistel (1978) report that children often restart counting when they skip an object in the set or a number word. Another study shows that when math problems are framed nonsymbolically using two magic cups that add different numbers of items to the original set, children can infer from which of the two cups the new items came from (i.e., an addendunknown problem; Kibbe & Feigenson, 2015). In both of these situations, children demonstrated the basic ability to compare the expected outcome with the actual one and use that error information.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For instance, Gelman and Gallistel (1978) report that children often restart counting when they skip an object in the set or a number word. Another study shows that when math problems are framed nonsymbolically using two magic cups that add different numbers of items to the original set, children can infer from which of the two cups the new items came from (i.e., an addendunknown problem; Kibbe & Feigenson, 2015). In both of these situations, children demonstrated the basic ability to compare the expected outcome with the actual one and use that error information.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We extend the debate to algebra and demonstrate nuance in the relation between ANS acuity and fact memory and individual differences in this area (see also Kibbe & Feigenson, 2015). …”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 53%
“…The ANS is an inherent system for representing, comparing, and combining the magnitudes of collections of objects (see Feigenson, Dehaene, & Spelke, 2004; Geary, Berch, & Mann Koepke, 2015), and there is some evidence that poor acuity of this system contributes to difficulties in learning mathematics (Piazza et al, 2010), and to individual differences in mathematics achievement more generally (Chen & Li, 2014; Fazio, Bailey, Thompson, & Siegler, 2014; Kibbe & Feigenson, 2015; Libertus, Halberda, & Feigenson, 2011; Starr, Libertus, & Brannon, 2013). Other studies, however, suggest that children’s and adults’ formal mathematical competencies, whether or not they have learning difficulties, are largely independent of ANS acuity and that individual differences in mathematics achievement are more consistently related to the fluency of processing symbolic numerical and arithmetical information (e.g., Bugden & Ansari, 2011; De Smedt et al, 2011; De Smedt, Noël, Gilmore, & Ansari, 2013; Iuculano, Tang, Hall, & Butterworth, 2008; Rousselle & Noël, 2007) or to more basic processes, such as inhibitory control that influence performance on both ANS tasks and mathematics achievement tests (Fuhs & McNeil, 2013; Gilmore et al, 2013; but see Keller & Libertus, 2015).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Previous research has demonstrated that children can solve math problems non-symbolically and approximately before they comprehend the same operations symbolically (Barth et al, 2005 ). With the ANS, young children can compare, add, subtract, multiply, and divide, and solve simple linear equations using sets of objects with ratio-dependent precision (Barth et al, 2006 ; McCrink and Spelke, 2010 , 2016 ; Kibbe and Feigenson, 2015 ). In contrast to these prodigious non-symbolic and approximate mathematical abilities, children must be explicitly taught how to solve the same symbolic mathematical problems effectively over years of formal schooling.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%